Matches in SemOpenAlex for { <https://semopenalex.org/work/W834630007> ?p ?o ?g. }
Showing items 1 to 75 of
75
with 100 items per page.
- W834630007 endingPage "1970" @default.
- W834630007 startingPage "1965" @default.
- W834630007 abstract "Emotional Valence is Body-Specific: Evidence from spontaneous gestures during US presidential debates. Daniel Casasanto Kyle Jasmin (daniel.casasanto@mpi.nl) (kyle.jasmin@mpi.nl) Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics Wundtlaan 1, 6525 XD Nijmegen, The Netherlands Abstract emotional valence that is not encoded in any known language or cultural artifacts, but which was predicted based on particulars of our bodies. What is the relationship between motor action and emotion? Here we investigated whether people associate good things more strongly with the dominant side of their bodies, and bad things with the non-dominant side. To find out, we analyzed spontaneous gestures during speech expressing ideas with positive or negative emotional valence (e.g., freedom, pain, compassion). Samples of speech and gesture were drawn from the 2004 and 2008 US presidential debates, which involved two left-handers (Obama, McCain) and two right- handers (Kerry, Bush). Results showed a strong association between the valence of spoken clauses and the hands used to make spontaneous co-speech gestures. In right-handed candidates, right-hand gestures were more strongly associated with positive-valence clauses, and left-hand gestures with negative-valence clauses. Left-handed candidates showed the opposite pattern. Right- and left-handers implicitly associated positive valence more strongly with their dominant hand: the hand they can use more fluently. These results support the body-specificity hypothesis, (Casasanto, 2009), and suggest a perceptuomotor basis for even our most abstract ideas. Keywords: Body-specificity hypothesis; Gesture; Handedness; Metaphor; Presidential election; Valence Introduction Language and culture are two powerful forces that shape our minds. Where languages and cultures differ from one another, linguistic and cultural experience gives rise to language-specific and culture-specific patterns of thinking and acting, plausibly via ordinary learning mechanisms (e.g., Casasanto, 2008a; Nisbett, Peng, Choi, & Norenzayan, 2001; cf., Fodor, 1985; Pinker, 1994). Here we investigate another pervasive force that shapes our thoughts, feelings, and actions: bodily experience. According to the body-specificity hypothesis (Casasanto, 2009), people with different kinds of bodies, who interact with their physical environments in systematically different ways, should form correspondingly different ‘body-specific’ mental representations. It is challenging to disentangle the potential contributions of linguistic, cultural, and bodily experience to the structure of our minds. Because patterns in language and culture closely mirror patterns of bodily interactions with the environment (Clark, 1973), language, culture, and body generally make the same neural and behavioral predictions. To overcome this obstacle, the present study tested for a body-specific association between physical space and In language and culture, Good=Right. Across languages and cultures, left is conventionally associated with bad and right with good. English idioms like the right answer and my right hand man link good things with rightward space, and complementary idioms like out in left field and two left feet associate bad things with leftward space. The Latin words for right and left, dexter and sinister, form the roots of English words meaning skillful and evil, respectively. The words for right in French (droite) and in German (Recht) are closely related to the words meaning a ‘right’ or privilege accorded by the law, whereas the words for left in French (gauche) and German (Links) are related to words meaning distasteful or clumsy. Left-right idioms are also evident in nonlinguistic conventions in many cultures. Roman orators were admonished never to gesture with their left hand, alone (Quintillian, Institutio Oratoria, Book 11). Actors in the English renaissance were warned that vigorous left-hand gesturing was not only vulgar but also dangerous, and could result in the ‘vital spirits’ exploding out of the left ventricle (Bulwer, Chironomia, 1644). In modern Ghanaian society, pointing and gesturing with the left hand is prohibited (Kita & Essegby, 2001). According to Islamic doctrine, the left hand should only be used for dirty jobs like cleaning one’s self, whereas the right hand is used for eating. Likewise, the left foot is used for stepping into the bathroom, and the right foot for entering the mosque. Why does good correspond to right and bad to left, throughout the world and throughout the ages? One possible explanation is that this pattern arises from universal properties of the human brain and mind, perhaps related to innate hemispheric specialization for approach and avoidance motivational systems (Maxwell & Davidson, 2007). Once established due to innate neurobiological factors, conventions in language and culture may reinforce this implicit preference for the right. An alternative possibility, however, is that left-right conventions in language and culture arise as a consequence of body-specific associations between space and valence. Bodies are lopsided. Most people have a dominant hand, usually the right hand (Corballis & Beale, 1976), and therefore interact with their environment more fluently on" @default.
- W834630007 created "2016-06-24" @default.
- W834630007 creator A5043129720 @default.
- W834630007 creator A5086958518 @default.
- W834630007 date "2009-01-01" @default.
- W834630007 modified "2023-09-23" @default.
- W834630007 title "Emotional valence is body-specific: Evidence from spontaneous gestures during US presidential debates" @default.
- W834630007 cites W1520020354 @default.
- W834630007 cites W1989584280 @default.
- W834630007 cites W2030617327 @default.
- W834630007 cites W2031479363 @default.
- W834630007 cites W2097562465 @default.
- W834630007 cites W2113352630 @default.
- W834630007 cites W2118163921 @default.
- W834630007 cites W2129270174 @default.
- W834630007 cites W2144712959 @default.
- W834630007 cites W2155252468 @default.
- W834630007 cites W2163239204 @default.
- W834630007 cites W2323385789 @default.
- W834630007 cites W2905472553 @default.
- W834630007 cites W3125656407 @default.
- W834630007 hasPublicationYear "2009" @default.
- W834630007 type Work @default.
- W834630007 sameAs 834630007 @default.
- W834630007 citedByCount "1" @default.
- W834630007 countsByYear W8346300072013 @default.
- W834630007 crossrefType "journal-article" @default.
- W834630007 hasAuthorship W834630007A5043129720 @default.
- W834630007 hasAuthorship W834630007A5086958518 @default.
- W834630007 hasConcept C138885662 @default.
- W834630007 hasConcept C15744967 @default.
- W834630007 hasConcept C168900304 @default.
- W834630007 hasConcept C169760540 @default.
- W834630007 hasConcept C169900460 @default.
- W834630007 hasConcept C178790620 @default.
- W834630007 hasConcept C180747234 @default.
- W834630007 hasConcept C185592680 @default.
- W834630007 hasConcept C207347870 @default.
- W834630007 hasConcept C2778311575 @default.
- W834630007 hasConcept C3020774634 @default.
- W834630007 hasConcept C41895202 @default.
- W834630007 hasConcept C46312422 @default.
- W834630007 hasConcept C77805123 @default.
- W834630007 hasConcept C89267518 @default.
- W834630007 hasConceptScore W834630007C138885662 @default.
- W834630007 hasConceptScore W834630007C15744967 @default.
- W834630007 hasConceptScore W834630007C168900304 @default.
- W834630007 hasConceptScore W834630007C169760540 @default.
- W834630007 hasConceptScore W834630007C169900460 @default.
- W834630007 hasConceptScore W834630007C178790620 @default.
- W834630007 hasConceptScore W834630007C180747234 @default.
- W834630007 hasConceptScore W834630007C185592680 @default.
- W834630007 hasConceptScore W834630007C207347870 @default.
- W834630007 hasConceptScore W834630007C2778311575 @default.
- W834630007 hasConceptScore W834630007C3020774634 @default.
- W834630007 hasConceptScore W834630007C41895202 @default.
- W834630007 hasConceptScore W834630007C46312422 @default.
- W834630007 hasConceptScore W834630007C77805123 @default.
- W834630007 hasConceptScore W834630007C89267518 @default.
- W834630007 hasIssue "31" @default.
- W834630007 hasLocation W8346300071 @default.
- W834630007 hasOpenAccess W834630007 @default.
- W834630007 hasPrimaryLocation W8346300071 @default.
- W834630007 hasRelatedWork W2127427168 @default.
- W834630007 hasRelatedWork W2309652428 @default.
- W834630007 hasRelatedWork W3125656407 @default.
- W834630007 hasRelatedWork W45608174 @default.
- W834630007 hasRelatedWork W571117518 @default.
- W834630007 hasVolume "31" @default.
- W834630007 isParatext "false" @default.
- W834630007 isRetracted "false" @default.
- W834630007 magId "834630007" @default.
- W834630007 workType "article" @default.