Matches in SemOpenAlex for { <https://semopenalex.org/work/W137444494> ?p ?o ?g. }
Showing items 1 to 87 of
87
with 100 items per page.
- W137444494 abstract "The Game Lies in the Eye of the Beholder: The Influence of Expertise on Watching Soccer Michael Smuc (michael.smuc@donau-uni.ac.at) Eva Mayr (eva.mayr@donau-uni.ac.at) Florian Windhager (florian.windhager@donau-uni.ac.at) Research Center KnowComm, Danube University Krems, Dr.-Karl-Dorrek-Str. 30 3500 Krems, Austria Abstract The influence of expertise on viewing soccer matches is already an area of extensive research focusing on training. However, free viewing of soccer matches did receive less attention. In an explorative eye-tracking study we compared the viewing behavior of novices, amateur players, and professional players watching soccer scenes freely. Overall, novices seem to view a soccer match quite similar to professional players, whereas amateurs engage in more visual work. The viewing behavior differs when watching soccer freely or with a task in mind – a result worth a second glance. Keywords: Soccer, viewing behavior, eye tracking, expertise Introduction Watching soccer without the corresponding experience and domain knowledge is a real challenge. Without knowledge about standard situations and tactical behavior, an unskilled observer is restricted to following the ball’s trajectory mainly. On the other extreme, a good commentator is able to take in the whole scene at once and comment on the events and possible next moves. But what is it that enables an experienced soccer viewer to direct his or her attention more strategically and to take in more relevant information in comparison to an inexperienced one? To answer this question, we review existing research on eye-movements in sports and their relation to expertise. We present a study that compares the television viewing behavior of soccer laypersons, amateurs, and professional players. Eye-Movements in Watching Television In general, viewing television is a complex activity (Josephson & Holmes, 2006): A huge amount of information has to be processed at a speed, which cannot be controlled by the viewer. Kirkorian (2007) assumes that watching television is nearest to perceiving scenes (e.g., Henderson, 2007). Both convey complex visual stimuli, but instead of viewing only one scene, television includes a series of static frames. To examine visual information processing, eye tracking technology provides a means to observe a viewer’s point-of- gaze (e.g., Rayner, 1998). In the past, eye tracking focused mainly on scene perception and reading under laboratory conditions (Henderson, 2007; Rayner, 1998); only in the last years, applications in more everyday settings (e.g., Hayhoe & Ballard, 2005; Mayr, Knipfer, & Wessel, 2009) became possible with the emergence of more usable technology. Central eye-movement measures are fixations and saccades. Saccades are shifts from one point of gaze to another; fixations indicate visual attention to that information (Rayner, 1998). In scene perception, top-down and bottom-up influences control where one looks (Henderson, 2007). Bottom-up influences are stimulus- driven, whereas top-down influences are viewer-driven. Bottom-up influences are mainly based on the visual salience of the stimulus, i.e., color, saturation, and – which is especially important in television – movement (Mahapatra, Winkler, & Yen, 2008). Also, research on eye- movements during film watching shows that a high degree of the fixations is within the center of the screen (Goldstein, Woods, & Peli, 2007). An open question is whether this is due to a trend to fixate the center or due to movie making conventions placing the most relevant information in the center of the screen. Top-down influences on the other hand are a viewer’s knowledge about the stimulus, his or her domain knowledge, and his or her goals (Henderson, 2003). It was shown that expectations about camera angles, cuts and close-ups determine television viewing behavior (Kirkorian, 2007). These expectations are learned and, therefore, get stronger with viewing experience. Another top-down influence is the viewer’s domain knowledge. Chase and Simon (1973) showed that due to their higher knowledge on possible configurations experts in chess can easier create chunks of information. A similar mechanism can be assumed in soccer experts and was already shown to be influential (Ward & Williams, 2003). A third top-down influence is the existence of specific goals. Only little research exists on humans watching television freely, i.e. without any task or instruction (see Goldstein et al., 2007, for an exception). However, Spanne (2006) showed that similar to viewing natural scenes (DeAngelus & Pelz, 2009) viewing behavior of movies differs according to the task at hand and in free viewing. But until now no research on free viewing behavior in soccer exists. Rather, most research asked players to anticipate the next move, recall the players’ positions (e.g., Ward & Williams, 2003), or actively pass the ball (Helsen & Starkes, As watching soccer for leisure purposes is a free viewing condition, it has to be questioned whether existing research" @default.
- W137444494 created "2016-06-24" @default.
- W137444494 creator A5056931882 @default.
- W137444494 creator A5079730303 @default.
- W137444494 creator A5084024959 @default.
- W137444494 date "2010-01-01" @default.
- W137444494 modified "2023-10-03" @default.
- W137444494 title "The Game Lies in the Eye of the Beholder: The Influence of Expertise on Watching Soccer" @default.
- W137444494 cites W110979171 @default.
- W137444494 cites W1713455327 @default.
- W137444494 cites W1977145833 @default.
- W137444494 cites W1980315708 @default.
- W137444494 cites W2012970160 @default.
- W137444494 cites W2013112874 @default.
- W137444494 cites W2041306383 @default.
- W137444494 cites W2044834272 @default.
- W137444494 cites W2049389369 @default.
- W137444494 cites W2056769301 @default.
- W137444494 cites W2077226294 @default.
- W137444494 cites W2091785129 @default.
- W137444494 cites W2094187780 @default.
- W137444494 cites W2115962615 @default.
- W137444494 cites W2118965714 @default.
- W137444494 cites W2119486501 @default.
- W137444494 cites W2119976713 @default.
- W137444494 cites W2132779202 @default.
- W137444494 cites W2147299944 @default.
- W137444494 cites W2210340326 @default.
- W137444494 cites W2745869042 @default.
- W137444494 hasPublicationYear "2010" @default.
- W137444494 type Work @default.
- W137444494 sameAs 137444494 @default.
- W137444494 citedByCount "1" @default.
- W137444494 countsByYear W1374444942012 @default.
- W137444494 crossrefType "journal-article" @default.
- W137444494 hasAuthorship W137444494A5056931882 @default.
- W137444494 hasAuthorship W137444494A5079730303 @default.
- W137444494 hasAuthorship W137444494A5084024959 @default.
- W137444494 hasConcept C153050134 @default.
- W137444494 hasConcept C154945302 @default.
- W137444494 hasConcept C15744967 @default.
- W137444494 hasConcept C169760540 @default.
- W137444494 hasConcept C17744445 @default.
- W137444494 hasConcept C199539241 @default.
- W137444494 hasConcept C2778044066 @default.
- W137444494 hasConcept C41008148 @default.
- W137444494 hasConcept C56461940 @default.
- W137444494 hasConcept C77805123 @default.
- W137444494 hasConceptScore W137444494C153050134 @default.
- W137444494 hasConceptScore W137444494C154945302 @default.
- W137444494 hasConceptScore W137444494C15744967 @default.
- W137444494 hasConceptScore W137444494C169760540 @default.
- W137444494 hasConceptScore W137444494C17744445 @default.
- W137444494 hasConceptScore W137444494C199539241 @default.
- W137444494 hasConceptScore W137444494C2778044066 @default.
- W137444494 hasConceptScore W137444494C41008148 @default.
- W137444494 hasConceptScore W137444494C56461940 @default.
- W137444494 hasConceptScore W137444494C77805123 @default.
- W137444494 hasIssue "32" @default.
- W137444494 hasLocation W1374444941 @default.
- W137444494 hasOpenAccess W137444494 @default.
- W137444494 hasPrimaryLocation W1374444941 @default.
- W137444494 hasRelatedWork W1531179510 @default.
- W137444494 hasRelatedWork W1753356130 @default.
- W137444494 hasRelatedWork W1998129753 @default.
- W137444494 hasRelatedWork W2006486802 @default.
- W137444494 hasRelatedWork W2015843449 @default.
- W137444494 hasRelatedWork W2015947491 @default.
- W137444494 hasRelatedWork W2048961916 @default.
- W137444494 hasRelatedWork W2063208830 @default.
- W137444494 hasRelatedWork W2093080027 @default.
- W137444494 hasRelatedWork W2098047046 @default.
- W137444494 hasRelatedWork W2149923274 @default.
- W137444494 hasRelatedWork W2166766547 @default.
- W137444494 hasRelatedWork W2566632352 @default.
- W137444494 hasRelatedWork W2616926580 @default.
- W137444494 hasRelatedWork W2794885176 @default.
- W137444494 hasRelatedWork W2798867139 @default.
- W137444494 hasRelatedWork W3092329399 @default.
- W137444494 hasRelatedWork W3160596880 @default.
- W137444494 hasRelatedWork W3193747054 @default.
- W137444494 hasRelatedWork W2963318143 @default.
- W137444494 hasVolume "32" @default.
- W137444494 isParatext "false" @default.
- W137444494 isRetracted "false" @default.
- W137444494 magId "137444494" @default.
- W137444494 workType "article" @default.