Matches in SemOpenAlex for { <https://semopenalex.org/work/W138782007> ?p ?o ?g. }
Showing items 1 to 72 of
72
with 100 items per page.
- W138782007 abstract "Language specific preferences in anaphor resolution: Exposure or Gricean maxims? Barbara Hemforth (barbara.hemforth@parisdescartes.fr) Laboratoire de Psychologie et de Neuropsychologie Cognitives, CNRS, Universite Paris Descartes, 71 ave Edouard Vaillant, 92100 Boulogne-Billancourt, France Lars Konieczny (lars@cognition.uni-freiburg.de) Center for Cognitive Science, University of Freiburg, Friedrichstr. 50, 79098 Freiburg, Germany Christoph Scheepers (c.scheepers@psy.gla.ac.uk) Department of Psychology, University of Glasgow, 58 Hillhead Street, Glasgow, Scotland Saveria Colonna (Saveria.Colonna@univ-paris8.fr) Laboratoire Structure Formelles du Langage, CNRS, Universite Paris 8, 59-61 rue Pouchet, 75849 Paris Cedex 17 Sarah Schimke (sarah.schimke@sfl.cnrs.fr) Laboratoire Structure Formelles du Langage, CNRS, Universite Paris 8, 59-61 rue Pouchet, 75849 Paris Cedex 17 Peter Baumann (p.bau@web.de) Center for Cognitive Science, University of Freiburg, Friedrichstr. 50, 79098 Freiburg, Germany Joel Pynte (joel.pynte@parisdescartes.fr) Laboratoire de Psychologie et de Neuropsychologie Cognitives, CNRS, Universite Paris Descartes, 71 ave Edouard Vaillant, 92100 Boulogne-Billancourt, France Abstract In this paper we will present evidence for language specific preferences in anaphor resolution from two series of experiments in English, German, and French. For within sentence anaphor resolution with “before” subclauses, we will show that English and German follow the generally assumed preference for the first mentioned NP or subject of the sentence, whereas French shows a clear preference for the object of the matrix clause. We will argue that our data can most easily be explained by a usage-based account, linking comprehension preferences to production preferences. Keywords: Sentence processing; anaphor resolution; crosslinguistic differences; usage-based preferences Introduction It has been shown for many languages that the resolution of non-reflexive pronouns is strongly influenced by pragmatic factors such as topicality (in the sentence or in the discourse; Givon, 1983), the chain of causality, and other kinds of discourse relations (e.g. Kehler, 2002; Sanders & Noordman, 2000). On the sentence level, two of the factors that seem to play a role are a preference for the first mentioned antecedent (Gernsbacher, 1990), and a preference for the subject (Jaervikivi, van Gompel, Hyona, & Bertram, 2005). These preferences are assumed to be valid across languages so that for subject-verb-object sentences like (1) a preference for the first noun phrase would generally be predicted, given that it is mentioned first and the subject at the same time. English: The postman met the streetsweeper before he went home. French: Le facteur a rencontre le balayeur avant qu'il rentre a la maison. German: Der Brieftrager hat den Strassenfeger getroffen bevor er nach Hause ging. More language specific predictions can be derived from accounts based on the availability of alternative constructions in the grammar of a particular language. According to the Gricean Maxim of Manner (Clarity), speakers should avoid ambiguous constructions in choosing unambiguous alternatives if they exist. If for an ambiguous construction an unambigous alternative exists for one of the readings, listeners may thus assume that the speaker would have chosen this alternative for the respective reading. From this reasoning, a preference for the reading without an unambiguous alternative will result for the ambiguous construction. In this paper, we will compare closely matched constructions in English, French, and German (see examples 2-5) to investigate cross-linguistic differences in pronoun resolution. What makes the comparison of these languages particularly interesting, is the distribution of alternative constructions for the different interpretations of an ambiguous sentence like (1): In French, a highly frequent construction exists for binding an anaphoric pronoun to the subject of the matrix clause (2) which does not exist for German." @default.
- W138782007 created "2016-06-24" @default.
- W138782007 creator A5012115124 @default.
- W138782007 creator A5021186422 @default.
- W138782007 creator A5022150629 @default.
- W138782007 creator A5031073373 @default.
- W138782007 creator A5036432253 @default.
- W138782007 creator A5049814828 @default.
- W138782007 creator A5061634175 @default.
- W138782007 date "2010-01-01" @default.
- W138782007 modified "2023-10-17" @default.
- W138782007 title "Language specific preferences in anaphor resolution: Exposure or gricean maxims?" @default.
- W138782007 cites W1556417835 @default.
- W138782007 cites W1568886291 @default.
- W138782007 cites W1586232984 @default.
- W138782007 cites W1604686258 @default.
- W138782007 cites W2006482224 @default.
- W138782007 cites W2012086511 @default.
- W138782007 cites W2053573199 @default.
- W138782007 cites W2067527300 @default.
- W138782007 cites W2110689058 @default.
- W138782007 cites W2129390449 @default.
- W138782007 cites W2157467951 @default.
- W138782007 cites W2169985884 @default.
- W138782007 hasPublicationYear "2010" @default.
- W138782007 type Work @default.
- W138782007 sameAs 138782007 @default.
- W138782007 citedByCount "6" @default.
- W138782007 countsByYear W1387820072012 @default.
- W138782007 countsByYear W1387820072013 @default.
- W138782007 countsByYear W1387820072014 @default.
- W138782007 countsByYear W1387820072018 @default.
- W138782007 crossrefType "book" @default.
- W138782007 hasAuthorship W138782007A5012115124 @default.
- W138782007 hasAuthorship W138782007A5021186422 @default.
- W138782007 hasAuthorship W138782007A5022150629 @default.
- W138782007 hasAuthorship W138782007A5031073373 @default.
- W138782007 hasAuthorship W138782007A5036432253 @default.
- W138782007 hasAuthorship W138782007A5049814828 @default.
- W138782007 hasAuthorship W138782007A5061634175 @default.
- W138782007 hasBestOaLocation W1387820071 @default.
- W138782007 hasConcept C138268822 @default.
- W138782007 hasConcept C138885662 @default.
- W138782007 hasConcept C154945302 @default.
- W138782007 hasConcept C15744967 @default.
- W138782007 hasConcept C204321447 @default.
- W138782007 hasConcept C41008148 @default.
- W138782007 hasConcept C41895202 @default.
- W138782007 hasConceptScore W138782007C138268822 @default.
- W138782007 hasConceptScore W138782007C138885662 @default.
- W138782007 hasConceptScore W138782007C154945302 @default.
- W138782007 hasConceptScore W138782007C15744967 @default.
- W138782007 hasConceptScore W138782007C204321447 @default.
- W138782007 hasConceptScore W138782007C41008148 @default.
- W138782007 hasConceptScore W138782007C41895202 @default.
- W138782007 hasLocation W1387820071 @default.
- W138782007 hasOpenAccess W138782007 @default.
- W138782007 hasPrimaryLocation W1387820071 @default.
- W138782007 hasRelatedWork W1552159754 @default.
- W138782007 hasRelatedWork W2148757832 @default.
- W138782007 hasRelatedWork W2293457016 @default.
- W138782007 hasRelatedWork W2368651715 @default.
- W138782007 hasRelatedWork W2611614995 @default.
- W138782007 hasRelatedWork W2748952813 @default.
- W138782007 hasRelatedWork W2789919619 @default.
- W138782007 hasRelatedWork W2899084033 @default.
- W138782007 hasRelatedWork W3107474891 @default.
- W138782007 hasRelatedWork W4321496520 @default.
- W138782007 isParatext "false" @default.
- W138782007 isRetracted "false" @default.
- W138782007 magId "138782007" @default.
- W138782007 workType "book" @default.