Matches in SemOpenAlex for { <https://semopenalex.org/work/W1524975056> ?p ?o ?g. }
Showing items 1 to 62 of
62
with 100 items per page.
- W1524975056 abstract "How can an athlete regulate the rate of fatigue accumulation during a time trial competition? How is muscle contractile activity regulated to maintain the highest work rate possible without precipitating fatigue precociously? At the end of exercise will the level of central fatigue coincide with the level of peripheral fatigue, even if the conditions of exercise have changed dramatically? Previous investigations have used either ‘open-loop’ designs in which the influence of a given factor on fatigue is studied by measuring its effect on endurance time (time to exhaustion) or ‘closed-loop’ designs in which power output and time to task completion is assessed. In this issue of The Journal of Physiology, Amann et al. (2006) use different levels of arterial oxygen content (CaO2) to manipulate the rate of fatigue accumulation during ‘open-’ and ‘closed-loop’ tasks. With this unique experimental approach Amann et al. (2006) show that peripheral locomotor muscle fatigue development is a significant determinant of the magnitude of central motor output (central command) during exercise. This is demonstrated by showing that during exercise at a given fixed intensity, the lower the CaO2, the greater was the rate of development of peripheral fatigue and the greater the time-dependent rise in the integrated electromyography (iEMG). During the time trial performance test (‘closed-loop’) in which the subject tries to cover a given distance in the shortest time possible, the iEMG is enhanced during the conditions with high CaO2 and vice versa. A unique aspect of the study is that at exhaustion, the degree of peripheral fatigue was the same in all conditions meaning that the CNS should receive information which then is used to up- or down-regulate motor output, and to optimize the rate of fatigue accumulation to the exercise conditions. This allows a delay in the attainment of the level of fatigue that would impede the continuation of exercise (Gandevia, 2001).With this study the group led by Jerome Dempsey makes an important contribution showing that fatigue is a signal useful to regulate motor output in order to optimize the rate of fatigue accumulation to the available functional reserve (capacity for exercise). Thus, from a classical view in which fatigue is perceived as something negative and catastrophic Amann et al. (2006) present a new vision in which peripheral muscle fatigue acts as a signal required for the optimal regulation of exercise intensity in order to attain the greatest level of performance achievable.These findings, however, should not be interpreted as meaning that fatigue, i.e. any exercise-induced reduction in the ability to exert muscle force or power, regardless of whether or not the task can be sustained (as it was defined by Bigland-Ritchie & Woods, 1984) is caused by peripheral sensory feedback. For example, at the end of an incremental exercise to exhaustion in severe acute hypoxia, but also in chronic hypoxia, the incremental exercise can be continued by allowing the subjects to breath a normoxic (or mild hyperoxic) gas mixture (Calbet et al. 2003a,b). Both conditions (acute and chronic hypoxia) are rather different. In severe acute hypoxia CaO2 is rather low but in chronic hypoxia maximal exercise CaO2 is the same as during normoxia without altitude acclimatization. According to Amann et al. (2006), neuromuscular fatigue should be similar at the end of an incremental exercise regardless of the inspired O2 fraction (FIO2). Nevertheless the studies by Calbet et al. indicate that in severe acute and chronic hypoxia fatigue can be easily overcome by raising arterial PO2, something that is not possible when subjects perform the incremental exercise in normoxia and on exhaustion breath hyperoxic gas (Calbet J, Boushel R and Lundby C, unpublished observations). How can these divergent findings be reconciled? Potentially hypoxia may attenuate the metabolically elicited sensory feedback causing fatigue (this will fit with the observation of a higher accumulation of muscle lactate during all-out exercise with hypoxia). Moreover, during sprint-like exercise to exhaustion of short duration, such as a Wingate test (Calbet et al. 2003c) exercise performance is not affected by moderate or severe hypoxia, despite the fact that anaerobic energy release is increased during the hypoxic sprint to compensate for the reduction of the aerobic ATP production. Why is fatigue not occurring sooner during an all-out exercise (sprint) with hypoxia, or during incremental exercise to exhaustion? One explanation could be that under these conditions sensory feedback is suppressed, blocked or simply counteracted by a stronger central command. Overall, these studies suggest that the central command during all-out testing may easily overcome sensory feedback signalling peripheral fatigue to the extreme of even increasing the accumulation of end products of the anaerobic metabolism beyond the limits normally reached during the same exercise conditions in normoxia.During an all-out test the subject aims at developing the maximal power output from the start to the end of the exercise and presumably sensory feedback is not critical to modulate motor output. During open-loop tasks in which the exercise intensity is maintained constant, sensory feedback is not needed to regulate motor output, as shown by Kjaer et al. (1999). These authors demonstrated that blocking sensory feedback with epidural anaesthesia during a constant intensity exercise to exhaustion (open-loop) has no repercussion on endurance time or on the accumulation of muscle metabolites at exhaustion. Moreover, if anything, epidural anaesthesia resulted in an enhancement of the rate of perceived exertion due to a small reduction of muscle strength. According to the findings of Amann et al. (2006) epidural anaesthesia should impede the regulation of fatigue accumulation during time trials.Niels Secher and colleagues have shown that for a given metabolic rate central command is increased and so is the rate of perceived exertion when the exercise is performed after partial curarization. In the latter case fatigue cannot be explained by metabolic factors alone. Curarization experiments are compatible with the existence of some feed-forward neural mechanism that triggers signals contributing to the perception of fatigue depending on the magnitude of central command.In summary, in this issue of The Journal of PhysiologyAmann et al. (2006) show that the rate of fatigue accumulation is a sensed variable useful to regulate central command during endurance events. Future studies should clarify how the signal is generated and sensed, what factors may modulate it and how it is integrated with other sensory and descending nervous signals." @default.
- W1524975056 created "2016-06-24" @default.
- W1524975056 creator A5022335309 @default.
- W1524975056 date "2006-09-06" @default.
- W1524975056 modified "2023-09-26" @default.
- W1524975056 title "The rate of fatigue accumulation as a sensed variable" @default.
- W1524975056 cites W1546019921 @default.
- W1524975056 cites W1966597560 @default.
- W1524975056 cites W2054117980 @default.
- W1524975056 cites W2109991109 @default.
- W1524975056 cites W2112718015 @default.
- W1524975056 cites W2165453261 @default.
- W1524975056 cites W2185140711 @default.
- W1524975056 doi "https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2006.116087" @default.
- W1524975056 hasPubMedCentralId "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/1995686" @default.
- W1524975056 hasPubMedId "https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/16840512" @default.
- W1524975056 hasPublicationYear "2006" @default.
- W1524975056 type Work @default.
- W1524975056 sameAs 1524975056 @default.
- W1524975056 citedByCount "20" @default.
- W1524975056 countsByYear W15249750562012 @default.
- W1524975056 countsByYear W15249750562015 @default.
- W1524975056 countsByYear W15249750562016 @default.
- W1524975056 countsByYear W15249750562021 @default.
- W1524975056 countsByYear W15249750562022 @default.
- W1524975056 crossrefType "journal-article" @default.
- W1524975056 hasAuthorship W1524975056A5022335309 @default.
- W1524975056 hasBestOaLocation W15249750562 @default.
- W1524975056 hasConcept C134306372 @default.
- W1524975056 hasConcept C182365436 @default.
- W1524975056 hasConcept C185592680 @default.
- W1524975056 hasConcept C33923547 @default.
- W1524975056 hasConcept C39432304 @default.
- W1524975056 hasConcept C71924100 @default.
- W1524975056 hasConcept C99508421 @default.
- W1524975056 hasConceptScore W1524975056C134306372 @default.
- W1524975056 hasConceptScore W1524975056C182365436 @default.
- W1524975056 hasConceptScore W1524975056C185592680 @default.
- W1524975056 hasConceptScore W1524975056C33923547 @default.
- W1524975056 hasConceptScore W1524975056C39432304 @default.
- W1524975056 hasConceptScore W1524975056C71924100 @default.
- W1524975056 hasConceptScore W1524975056C99508421 @default.
- W1524975056 hasLocation W15249750561 @default.
- W1524975056 hasLocation W15249750562 @default.
- W1524975056 hasLocation W15249750563 @default.
- W1524975056 hasLocation W15249750564 @default.
- W1524975056 hasOpenAccess W1524975056 @default.
- W1524975056 hasPrimaryLocation W15249750561 @default.
- W1524975056 hasRelatedWork W136259318 @default.
- W1524975056 hasRelatedWork W2354248364 @default.
- W1524975056 hasRelatedWork W2374021052 @default.
- W1524975056 hasRelatedWork W3007404728 @default.
- W1524975056 hasRelatedWork W3018365851 @default.
- W1524975056 hasRelatedWork W3123026918 @default.
- W1524975056 hasRelatedWork W3125961646 @default.
- W1524975056 hasRelatedWork W3207484021 @default.
- W1524975056 hasRelatedWork W77650111 @default.
- W1524975056 hasRelatedWork W3123600015 @default.
- W1524975056 isParatext "false" @default.
- W1524975056 isRetracted "false" @default.
- W1524975056 magId "1524975056" @default.
- W1524975056 workType "article" @default.