Matches in SemOpenAlex for { <https://semopenalex.org/work/W170783537> ?p ?o ?g. }
Showing items 1 to 83 of
83
with 100 items per page.
- W170783537 endingPage "158" @default.
- W170783537 startingPage "158" @default.
- W170783537 abstract "Students with attention deficit/hyperactive disorder (ADHD) prefer novelty and attend to salient features of stimuli, while disregarding other neutral, detailed, and often informative features of complex stimuli (e.g., Zentall, 1986; Zentall & Dwyer, 1988; Zentall, 1989). In contrast to this attentional style, it has been hypothesized that Spanish-speaking (S-S) bilingual students actually develop better than average visual abilities to compensate for their language differences (Baca & Cervantes, 1989; Cummins, 1979; 1984; De Avila & Duncan, 1977; Ramirez & Castaneda, 1974). That is, S-S students may rely on their visual skills to gain information during acquisition of a second language (Cummins, 1984; Krashen, 1985; Baruth & Manning, 1992). Baruth and Manning (1992) extended this line of reasoning by recommending that instructors present material in the visual-spatial mode and use visual images and symbols rather than definitions or verbal descriptions for S-S students. However, it is not known if these hypothesized differences are valid. We selected art representations rather than printed symbols, because children appear to have equivalent prior exposure to this type of information across cultures (Lankford, 1992; Mittler, 1994). Furthermore, the responses of children to artworks appear to follow a developmental progression. That is, younger students have a tendency to match paintings according to subject matter, thematic similarities, or colors (Suchman & Trabasso, 1966). During adolescent years, students overlook the identity of particular objects being represented and sort paintings based on less obvious aspects, such as style (i.e., realistic, semi-abstract, and abstract) (Gardner, 1970, 1972; Machotka, 1966; Parsons, 1987; Silverman, Winner, Rosenstiel & Gardner, 1975; Tighe, 1968; Walk, 1967). Although younger children may ignore style while sorting paintings, we predicted that they might recognize stylistic differences. In addition to assessing for possible differences in the preferences for paintings by S-S and English-speaking (E-S) children with and without ADHD, the major purpose of this study was to assess the responses of these students to a visual training program. This program was designed to focus their attention on the essential elements within paintings. Helping students attend to complex visual information may be particularly important, because of the relationship between length of time examining a visual stimulus and subsequent understanding of that stimulus (Gardner, 1990; Newton, 1989). Unfortunately, children with ADHD typically spend less time examining a stimulus, so strategies that help with rapid analysis of complex material would be useful. Their failure to attend may be even more evident in art. That is, even though students with ADHD might have considerable interest or skill in art, they appear to present considerable management problems to art educators (Zentall, Moon, Hall, & Grskovic, 2001). These management problems may also be related to prior experiences and expectations. Specialty-area educators (including art educators) reported that they had (a) tried and succeeded less often in altering their instructional methods for students with ADHD and (b) were more reluctant to attempt new techniques in the future (Stormont-Spurgin & Zentall, 1995). Thus, simple and effective techniques to focus attention may be particularly important for art educators. Predictions related to attention training were based on pre training performance. That is, if S-S students do rely on visual information, they would be expected to outperform E-S students on pre training measures of visual performance in response to viewing the paintings. Although we might anticipate that S-S students would perform optimally in response to visual information, students with S-S and ADHD might have greater initial difficulty if that information were complex because of their selective attention to salient stimulus characteristics, such as color. …" @default.
- W170783537 created "2016-06-24" @default.
- W170783537 creator A5006842238 @default.
- W170783537 creator A5014598718 @default.
- W170783537 creator A5062014842 @default.
- W170783537 date "2002-01-01" @default.
- W170783537 modified "2023-09-27" @default.
- W170783537 title "Responses to Art Attention-Training by English and Bilingual Spanish-Speaking Students with and without ADHD" @default.
- W170783537 cites W134053090 @default.
- W170783537 cites W1484864026 @default.
- W170783537 cites W1547939716 @default.
- W170783537 cites W1573872784 @default.
- W170783537 cites W1584789241 @default.
- W170783537 cites W1859245853 @default.
- W170783537 cites W1985814945 @default.
- W170783537 cites W1997163822 @default.
- W170783537 cites W2021054650 @default.
- W170783537 cites W2028566647 @default.
- W170783537 cites W2032669198 @default.
- W170783537 cites W2035280171 @default.
- W170783537 cites W2036947359 @default.
- W170783537 cites W2037659071 @default.
- W170783537 cites W2038470850 @default.
- W170783537 cites W2042169504 @default.
- W170783537 cites W2049414583 @default.
- W170783537 cites W2049863677 @default.
- W170783537 cites W2053459906 @default.
- W170783537 cites W2070004197 @default.
- W170783537 cites W2078886552 @default.
- W170783537 cites W2083353702 @default.
- W170783537 cites W2108104723 @default.
- W170783537 cites W2108836181 @default.
- W170783537 cites W2145684878 @default.
- W170783537 cites W2165722944 @default.
- W170783537 cites W2795156495 @default.
- W170783537 cites W367527110 @default.
- W170783537 cites W413120758 @default.
- W170783537 cites W624705326 @default.
- W170783537 cites W636639922 @default.
- W170783537 cites W97638625 @default.
- W170783537 cites W2296042795 @default.
- W170783537 doi "https://doi.org/10.2307/1321002" @default.
- W170783537 hasPublicationYear "2002" @default.
- W170783537 type Work @default.
- W170783537 sameAs 170783537 @default.
- W170783537 citedByCount "3" @default.
- W170783537 crossrefType "journal-article" @default.
- W170783537 hasAuthorship W170783537A5006842238 @default.
- W170783537 hasAuthorship W170783537A5014598718 @default.
- W170783537 hasAuthorship W170783537A5062014842 @default.
- W170783537 hasConcept C138885662 @default.
- W170783537 hasConcept C153294291 @default.
- W170783537 hasConcept C15744967 @default.
- W170783537 hasConcept C205649164 @default.
- W170783537 hasConcept C2777211547 @default.
- W170783537 hasConcept C41895202 @default.
- W170783537 hasConceptScore W170783537C138885662 @default.
- W170783537 hasConceptScore W170783537C153294291 @default.
- W170783537 hasConceptScore W170783537C15744967 @default.
- W170783537 hasConceptScore W170783537C205649164 @default.
- W170783537 hasConceptScore W170783537C2777211547 @default.
- W170783537 hasConceptScore W170783537C41895202 @default.
- W170783537 hasIssue "2" @default.
- W170783537 hasLocation W1707835371 @default.
- W170783537 hasOpenAccess W170783537 @default.
- W170783537 hasPrimaryLocation W1707835371 @default.
- W170783537 hasRelatedWork W100620283 @default.
- W170783537 hasRelatedWork W1530808388 @default.
- W170783537 hasRelatedWork W230091440 @default.
- W170783537 hasRelatedWork W258997015 @default.
- W170783537 hasRelatedWork W2748952813 @default.
- W170783537 hasRelatedWork W2810751659 @default.
- W170783537 hasRelatedWork W2899084033 @default.
- W170783537 hasRelatedWork W2997094352 @default.
- W170783537 hasRelatedWork W3216976533 @default.
- W170783537 hasRelatedWork W2233261550 @default.
- W170783537 hasVolume "43" @default.
- W170783537 isParatext "false" @default.
- W170783537 isRetracted "false" @default.
- W170783537 magId "170783537" @default.
- W170783537 workType "article" @default.