Matches in SemOpenAlex for { <https://semopenalex.org/work/W175048022> ?p ?o ?g. }
Showing items 1 to 97 of
97
with 100 items per page.
- W175048022 abstract "Exploring the Role of Verbal Category Labels in Flexible Cognition Jackson Tolins (jtolins@ucsc.edu) Department of Psychology, 1156 High St. Santa Cruz, CA 95064 USA Eliana Colunga (colunga@psych.colorado.edu) Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, 345 UCB Boulder, CO 80309 USA Abstract Research under the paradigm of the label feedback hypothesis has proposed a causal role for verbal labels in the online learning and processing of categories. Labeled categories are learned faster, and are subsequently more robust. The present study extends this research paradigm by considering the relationship between verbal labels and flexible categorization. Flexibility is a key trait of human cognition, and flexible categorization is important in a number of tasks. Participants learned to categorize ‘friendly’ and ‘unfriendly’ aliens either with or without names, followed by a transfer task. While selective attention to a particular dimension slowed relearning, no effect of label was found for either category learning or relearning with one exception; labels facilitated flexibility when selective attention was not involved in the transfer. The inability to replicate effects of verbal labels in category learning using similar methodologies raises interesting theoretical issues, questioning the extent to which this relationship applies. Keywords: Categorization; Label Feedback Hypothesis; Flexible Cognition; Selective Attention Introduction Language, along with use in communication, provides a symbolic system of representation through which a speaker contemplates the world around them. The emergence of the capacity for symbolic representation transformed human cognition (Deacon, 1997; DeLoache, 2004), permitting abstract thought and making possible cultural transmission of knowledge. Yet the relationship between language and other cognitive processes is still controversial. For many who view language as a distinct mental module (Gleitman & Papafragou, 2005; Pinker, 1995), language is merely a formal medium that is used to describe mental representations, while remaining independent of the concepts they express (Li & Gleitman, 2002). Recent work in understanding the relationship between language and thought has provided evidence against this disassociation. Instead, it has been suggested that language is best understood as built upon domain general cognitive processes, and thus potentially in a mutually transformative relationship with these processes (Bowerman & Choi, 2001; Gumperz & Levinson, 1996). With habitual use of the specific set of conceptual symbolic representations afforded by a language, an individual may be biased towards these representations in problem-solving and other cognitive tasks. How a language may accomplish this is not well understood. One possibility is that language reduces the ability to flexibly adjust categories outside the structure provided for by the words of a particular language. As such, it is important to consider the influence of language on the ability to dynamically activate and modify the cognitive process of categorization in response to changing task demands. The ability to think and act adaptively, while not a uniquely human trait, is a mental capacity uniquely well developed in human cognition and intelligent behavior (Deak, 2003). For the purposes of the current study, flexible cognition will be defined as a property of the cognitive system, rather than a specific mechanism or process (Deak, 2003; Ionescu, 2012). This definition allows for the consideration of flexible cognition in the interaction of interest; that between categorization and language, specifically verbal labels. Recent work lead by Gary Lupyan and colleagues on the role of labels in categorization has demonstrated a special status afforded to verbal labels (see e.g. Lupyan, Rakison, & McClelland, 2007; Lupyan & Thompson-Schill, 2011). Verbal labels participate in the learning of categories, facilitating learning, creating mental categories that are more robust than when the categories are learned without words (Lupyan, Rakison, & McClelland, 2007), and encouraging selective attention (Brojde, Porter, & Colunga, 2011). However, no study has looked directly at the influence of verbal labels on the perceptual and attentional processes that underlie flexibility after learning. Similarly, while a number of studies have looked at how language aides in an individual’s ability to flexibly adjust the level of categorization, or switch from taxonomic to thematic (Blaye, Bernard-Peyron, Paour, & Bonthoux, 2006), no previous research has investigated how individuals flexibly adjust their categorization strategies in regards to the same domain, on the same level. The present investigation seeks to illuminate further the relationship between verbal labels and the cognitive processes underlying categorization. In developing an understanding of the role that verbal labels play in the construction and maintenance of categories, we further our understanding of the relationship between language and the domain general cognitive processes, such as categorization, upon which language is built." @default.
- W175048022 created "2016-06-24" @default.
- W175048022 creator A5070748982 @default.
- W175048022 date "2012-01-01" @default.
- W175048022 modified "2023-09-26" @default.
- W175048022 title "Exploring the role of verbal category labels in flexible cognition" @default.
- W175048022 cites W1564017569 @default.
- W175048022 cites W1966647467 @default.
- W175048022 cites W1968153682 @default.
- W175048022 cites W1977486408 @default.
- W175048022 cites W1999260435 @default.
- W175048022 cites W2035122010 @default.
- W175048022 cites W2037838725 @default.
- W175048022 cites W2069098164 @default.
- W175048022 cites W2104964143 @default.
- W175048022 cites W2106425986 @default.
- W175048022 cites W2108969076 @default.
- W175048022 cites W2117061620 @default.
- W175048022 cites W2125306983 @default.
- W175048022 cites W2129668249 @default.
- W175048022 cites W2132089731 @default.
- W175048022 cites W2145997503 @default.
- W175048022 cites W2148300948 @default.
- W175048022 cites W2148754764 @default.
- W175048022 cites W2153564293 @default.
- W175048022 cites W2162004347 @default.
- W175048022 cites W2334905547 @default.
- W175048022 cites W2357427317 @default.
- W175048022 cites W2987688408 @default.
- W175048022 cites W2186582929 @default.
- W175048022 hasPublicationYear "2012" @default.
- W175048022 type Work @default.
- W175048022 sameAs 175048022 @default.
- W175048022 citedByCount "0" @default.
- W175048022 crossrefType "journal-article" @default.
- W175048022 hasAuthorship W175048022A5070748982 @default.
- W175048022 hasConcept C105795698 @default.
- W175048022 hasConcept C154945302 @default.
- W175048022 hasConcept C15744967 @default.
- W175048022 hasConcept C169760540 @default.
- W175048022 hasConcept C169900460 @default.
- W175048022 hasConcept C17744445 @default.
- W175048022 hasConcept C180747234 @default.
- W175048022 hasConcept C188147891 @default.
- W175048022 hasConcept C199539241 @default.
- W175048022 hasConcept C2776359362 @default.
- W175048022 hasConcept C2780598303 @default.
- W175048022 hasConcept C33923547 @default.
- W175048022 hasConcept C41008148 @default.
- W175048022 hasConcept C41690226 @default.
- W175048022 hasConcept C94124525 @default.
- W175048022 hasConcept C94625758 @default.
- W175048022 hasConceptScore W175048022C105795698 @default.
- W175048022 hasConceptScore W175048022C154945302 @default.
- W175048022 hasConceptScore W175048022C15744967 @default.
- W175048022 hasConceptScore W175048022C169760540 @default.
- W175048022 hasConceptScore W175048022C169900460 @default.
- W175048022 hasConceptScore W175048022C17744445 @default.
- W175048022 hasConceptScore W175048022C180747234 @default.
- W175048022 hasConceptScore W175048022C188147891 @default.
- W175048022 hasConceptScore W175048022C199539241 @default.
- W175048022 hasConceptScore W175048022C2776359362 @default.
- W175048022 hasConceptScore W175048022C2780598303 @default.
- W175048022 hasConceptScore W175048022C33923547 @default.
- W175048022 hasConceptScore W175048022C41008148 @default.
- W175048022 hasConceptScore W175048022C41690226 @default.
- W175048022 hasConceptScore W175048022C94124525 @default.
- W175048022 hasConceptScore W175048022C94625758 @default.
- W175048022 hasIssue "35" @default.
- W175048022 hasLocation W1750480221 @default.
- W175048022 hasOpenAccess W175048022 @default.
- W175048022 hasPrimaryLocation W1750480221 @default.
- W175048022 hasRelatedWork W1522575921 @default.
- W175048022 hasRelatedWork W1597919739 @default.
- W175048022 hasRelatedWork W1599146450 @default.
- W175048022 hasRelatedWork W168501255 @default.
- W175048022 hasRelatedWork W1926237779 @default.
- W175048022 hasRelatedWork W202487539 @default.
- W175048022 hasRelatedWork W2186238142 @default.
- W175048022 hasRelatedWork W2201577002 @default.
- W175048022 hasRelatedWork W2333475917 @default.
- W175048022 hasRelatedWork W2487287992 @default.
- W175048022 hasRelatedWork W2765487178 @default.
- W175048022 hasRelatedWork W2765971732 @default.
- W175048022 hasRelatedWork W2767489373 @default.
- W175048022 hasRelatedWork W2767919623 @default.
- W175048022 hasRelatedWork W2771684028 @default.
- W175048022 hasRelatedWork W2779998091 @default.
- W175048022 hasRelatedWork W3184114532 @default.
- W175048022 hasRelatedWork W579819891 @default.
- W175048022 hasRelatedWork W638246249 @default.
- W175048022 hasRelatedWork W2298895402 @default.
- W175048022 hasVolume "35" @default.
- W175048022 isParatext "false" @default.
- W175048022 isRetracted "false" @default.
- W175048022 magId "175048022" @default.
- W175048022 workType "article" @default.