Matches in SemOpenAlex for { <https://semopenalex.org/work/W1810226833> ?p ?o ?g. }
- W1810226833 abstract "We read with interest the commentary from Drs Salem and Moustafa (2015) on our paper and we would like to take the opportunity to further discuss two issues, namely, the use of chlorpromazine equivalents (CPZs) to control for the effects of antipsychotics and the need for longitudinal studies examining the complex relationships between cognition and antipsychotic-induced extrapyramidal symptoms (EPS) in schizophrenia.Drs Salem and Moustafa suggested calculating CPZs as means to rule out confounding dosage and variations in drugs. Although we agree with the notion put forward by the authors, especially in the context of a study investigating the side effects of antipsychotics, we would like to reiterate that there is yet to be a widely accepted robust antipsychotic dosage comparison system that reliably yields consistent equivalences across drugs (Patel et al., 2013). CPZs can be obtained via one of three calculations described by Patel et al. (2013): the mean effective dose in flexible-dose studies, the minimum effective dose, and the consensus of experts method. These methods yield inconsistent results particularly in the case of antipsychotics with low affinities for dopamine-D2 receptors (Sweileh et al., 2014). In our study, out of 82 schizophrenia-spectrum patients, 24 patients were treated with clozapine and 18 with quetiapine. In the literature, we note significant fluctuations in CPZs for quetiapine (60–175.5 mg/day) and clozapine (50–138.8 mg/day), an ambiguity highlighted when the two drugs are considered relative to one another (see Table S1 in Supplementary Material). In fact, depending on the CPZ calculation method, quetiapine will be considered more or less potent than clozapine. Therefore, even when only one equivalence system is employed, the comparison of antipsychotic medication dosage between studies for an individual drug is troublesome, and interpreting between-drug differences is problematic. The limited number of studies comparing directly atypical antipsychotics to chlorpromazine, an older typical antipsychotic, might explain some of the variability between the CPZ calculations. Nevertheless, when examining olanzapine equivalents, a system based on the availability of a larger set of randomized-controlled trials (RCTs) (Leucht et al., 2014, 2015), significant discrepancies between calculation methods of over 30% can be observed for both quetiapine (20–32.3 mg/day) and clozapine (30.6–40 mg/day) (see Table S1 in Supplementary Material). Other than CPZ, Daily Defined Doses (DDD) is a popular platform developed to compare drug utilization (Danivas and Venkatasubramanian, 2013). It is not, however, a measure of therapeutic efficacy like CPZ (Patel et al., 2013). Finally, equivalents of dopamine-D2 receptor occupancy appears as a relevant system amongst the remaining alternatives, as it targets a key neuropharmacological mechanism involved in the emergence of antipsychotic-induced EPS (de Greef et al., 2011). Indeed, several positron emission tomography studies have shown that antipsychotics are more likely to produce EPS at dosages associated with striatal D2 occupancy higher than 80% (Kapur and Seeman, 2001). However, the role of D2 receptors in the pathophysiology of EPS (dystonia, dyskinesia, akathisia) other than parkinsonism is less clear (Mehta et al., 2015). Moreover, antipsychotics bind several receptors other than D2 contributing to, or preventing, the emergence of EPS, such as dopamine D3, D4, serotonin 5-HT2A, and α1-adrenergic receptors (Horacek, 2000). For these reasons, the D2 equivalency method can be reductionist. In sum, until a consistent reliable system is developed and implemented, utilizing one dosage equivalence system versus another is likely to introduce significant caveats in the interpretation of experimental results, especially in the case of studies including patients treated with clozapine or quetiapine.The second issue that we wanted to raise concerns the fact that most studies investigating the relationship between cognition and EPS in schizophrenia are based on cross-sectional designs, which are more vulnerable to confounding factors than longitudinal designs. As rightly mentioned by Drs Salem and Moustafa, we sought to overcome this problem in our study by performing hierarchical multivariate linear regression analyses incorporating several socio-demographic and psychiatric variables likely to influence the cognition–EPS association (Potvin et al., 2015). Still, we feel that longitudinal studies are required in order to clarify the temporal dynamics of the association we observed between cognition (e.g., working memory) and drug-induced Parkinsonism. Interestingly, several RCTs have examined the effects of antipsychotics on cognition, which provide an opportunity to study the cognition–EPS association in a longitudinal fashion. The study of the cognition–EPS association is particularly relevant in this context, since it is theoretically possible that the presumed beneficial effects of second-generation antipsychotics (SGAs) on cognition are actually explained by the lower liability of these drugs to induce EPS, compared to first-generation antipsychotics (Leucht et al., 2013). It must also be considered that by causing fewer EPS, the treatment with SGAs renders the prescription of adjuvant anticholinergic drugs less necessary, the latter being well-known to impair cognition (Bubser et al., 2012). Recent meta-analyses have shown that the beneficial effects of SGAs (risperidone and olanzapine) on cognition are small-to-moderate (Desamericq et al., 2014). These benefits may not only reflect true effects but also be confounded by the effects of EPS and/or anticholinergics. Therefore, we reviewed 35 RCTs included in a recent meta-analysis from Nielsen et al. (2015) on the effects of antipsychotic on cognition in schizophrenia and systematically retrieved information on anticholinergics, EPS scales, EPS–cognition associations, and cognitive tests (see Table Table1).1). Of the 35 RCTs reviewed, 12 studies did not measure EPS. Of the remaining 23 studies, only 14 performed statistics on potential associations between EPS and cognition. Of these, six trials found an association between EPS and cognition, and eight did not. Although such results do not firmly establish a relationship between EPS and cognition, they do not rule it out either, especially since the mean duration of trials was 24 weeks, which may not be long enough to detect associations between EPS and cognition in schizophrenia. To our knowledge, the most reliable associations highlighted in cross-sectional studies are between cognition and tardive dyskinesia (Pantelis et al., 2001; Wu et al., 2013), which typically occurs later during treatment than akathisia, dystonia, and parkinsonism. Also of methodological concern, among the 35 RCTs reviewed, only 2 studies excluded anticholinergic drugs (Note: 8 trials did not report information on anticholinergics). Moreover, of the 25 remaining trials, only 9 studies examined potential associations between changes in cognitive performance and the prescription of adjuvant anticholinergics. At a time when the pharmacological treatment of cognitive deficits in schizophrenia is recognized as a top clinical priority, the lack of knowledge on the above-mentioned factors impedes our ability to reach valid conclusions on the presumed superior efficacy of SGAs (mostly risperidone and olanzapine) on cognition in schizophrenia. In return, these limitations undermine the ongoing debate about the pharmacological mechanisms that should be targeted to improve cognitive performance in schizophrenia.Table 1Review of the studies included in the meta-analysis from Nielsen et al. (2015)." @default.
- W1810226833 created "2016-06-24" @default.
- W1810226833 creator A5043353145 @default.
- W1810226833 creator A5049035422 @default.
- W1810226833 date "2015-08-12" @default.
- W1810226833 modified "2023-10-02" @default.
- W1810226833 title "Response to: A Commentary on “Antipsychotic-Induced Parkinsonism is Associated with Working Memory Deficits in Schizophrenia-Spectrum Disorders”" @default.
- W1810226833 cites W1685118621 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W1828753295 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W1967034969 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W1967377279 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W1970688947 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W1976741101 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W1983861222 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W1985065973 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W1985202323 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W1988586706 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2004782778 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2009832071 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2012504287 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2014616152 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2018753545 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2033027912 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2035393622 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2035897986 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2036966602 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2041495183 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2042051086 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2045865877 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2048815611 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2050045773 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2051026253 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2056212570 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2066246789 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2076623335 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2079666511 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2081162486 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2081291579 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2094188000 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2094598884 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2096001892 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2099576957 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2100405250 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2104198281 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2112167247 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2120300760 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2120330579 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2124221405 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2125653585 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2127549664 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2137628013 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2138795186 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2140249810 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2149805979 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2152832979 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2164207356 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2167173441 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2170792659 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W2170796105 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W4229750799 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W4230049912 @default.
- W1810226833 cites W4238675724 @default.
- W1810226833 doi "https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00210" @default.
- W1810226833 hasPubMedCentralId "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/4532927" @default.
- W1810226833 hasPubMedId "https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/26321931" @default.
- W1810226833 hasPublicationYear "2015" @default.
- W1810226833 type Work @default.
- W1810226833 sameAs 1810226833 @default.
- W1810226833 citedByCount "0" @default.
- W1810226833 crossrefType "journal-article" @default.
- W1810226833 hasAuthorship W1810226833A5043353145 @default.
- W1810226833 hasAuthorship W1810226833A5049035422 @default.
- W1810226833 hasBestOaLocation W18102268331 @default.
- W1810226833 hasConcept C118552586 @default.
- W1810226833 hasConcept C126322002 @default.
- W1810226833 hasConcept C15744967 @default.
- W1810226833 hasConcept C169760540 @default.
- W1810226833 hasConcept C169900460 @default.
- W1810226833 hasConcept C21963081 @default.
- W1810226833 hasConcept C2776412080 @default.
- W1810226833 hasConcept C2776525014 @default.
- W1810226833 hasConcept C2779134260 @default.
- W1810226833 hasConcept C2779727114 @default.
- W1810226833 hasConcept C2780494398 @default.
- W1810226833 hasConcept C3018432486 @default.
- W1810226833 hasConcept C71924100 @default.
- W1810226833 hasConceptScore W1810226833C118552586 @default.
- W1810226833 hasConceptScore W1810226833C126322002 @default.
- W1810226833 hasConceptScore W1810226833C15744967 @default.
- W1810226833 hasConceptScore W1810226833C169760540 @default.
- W1810226833 hasConceptScore W1810226833C169900460 @default.
- W1810226833 hasConceptScore W1810226833C21963081 @default.
- W1810226833 hasConceptScore W1810226833C2776412080 @default.
- W1810226833 hasConceptScore W1810226833C2776525014 @default.
- W1810226833 hasConceptScore W1810226833C2779134260 @default.
- W1810226833 hasConceptScore W1810226833C2779727114 @default.
- W1810226833 hasConceptScore W1810226833C2780494398 @default.
- W1810226833 hasConceptScore W1810226833C3018432486 @default.
- W1810226833 hasConceptScore W1810226833C71924100 @default.
- W1810226833 hasLocation W18102268331 @default.