Matches in SemOpenAlex for { <https://semopenalex.org/work/W1903378096> ?p ?o ?g. }
- W1903378096 endingPage "14" @default.
- W1903378096 startingPage "6" @default.
- W1903378096 abstract "The bulk of language usage is conversational, involving rapid exchange of turns. New information about the turn-taking system shows that this transition between speakers is generally more than threefold faster than language encoding. To maintain this pace of switching, participants must predict the content and timing of the incoming turn and begin language encoding as soon as possible, even while still processing the incoming turn. This intensive cognitive processing has been largely ignored by the language sciences because psycholinguistics has studied language production and comprehension separately from dialog. This fast pace holds across languages, and across modalities as in sign language. It is also evident in early infancy in ‘proto-conversation’ before infants control language. Turn-taking or ‘duetting’ has been observed in many other species and is found across all the major clades of the primate order. Most language usage is interactive, involving rapid turn-taking. The turn-taking system has a number of striking properties: turns are short and responses are remarkably rapid, but turns are of varying length and often of very complex construction such that the underlying cognitive processing is highly compressed. Although neglected in cognitive science, the system has deep implications for language processing and acquisition that are only now becoming clear. Appearing earlier in ontogeny than linguistic competence, it is also found across all the major primate clades. This suggests a possible phylogenetic continuity, which may provide key insights into language evolution. Most language usage is interactive, involving rapid turn-taking. The turn-taking system has a number of striking properties: turns are short and responses are remarkably rapid, but turns are of varying length and often of very complex construction such that the underlying cognitive processing is highly compressed. Although neglected in cognitive science, the system has deep implications for language processing and acquisition that are only now becoming clear. Appearing earlier in ontogeny than linguistic competence, it is also found across all the major primate clades. This suggests a possible phylogenetic continuity, which may provide key insights into language evolution. the shape of parsing trees representing the structure of sentences: a verb-final language such as Japanese is likely to have a left-branching structure, whereas a verb-initial language such as Welsh is likely to have a right branching structure which facilitates prediction (on encountering ‘ate’ one can expect an edible and an eater): a branch of sociology that, through careful observation, has shed much light on human interactional language use. socially-learned variety of a language or bird song. term used in studies of animal communication to denote the coordination in time of communication between partners (especially songbird pairs), often alternating in turns. the family-level clade (Hominidae) including Homo, Pan (chimpanzees and bonobos), Gorilla and Pongo (orangutans), but excluding the Hylobates (gibbons). the first hominin species to exit Africa and widely colonize Eurasia in the early Pleistocene, sometimes distinguished from the African variety Homo ergaster. in language production, the size of a unit that is encoded as a chunk; in subject-initial languages such as English or Japanese (see Branching structure, above) the initial increment can be as little as the subject noun phrase, in verb-initial languages such as Mayan or Welsh the initial increment must be the entire clause because the verb requires one, two, or more participants. the measurement of changes of volume of air, thus shedding light on breathing necessitated by speaking. the alternation of vocalization between mother and infant before language acquisition. the study of language use; pragmatic heuristics are systematic interpretative rules of thumb (e.g., a sequential interpretation of tensed conjoined clauses, as in ‘He came and saw it’). properties of speech of longer duration than segments (vowels and consonants), especially intonation, tone, stress, and rhythm. the study of communication and sign systems in the broadest sense, for example beyond language. the point or intention behind an utterance (e.g., a request vs an offer, a statement vs a question), sometimes termed illocutionary force. conversational turn-taking offers a basis for the elaboration of special turn-taking systems wherein, for example, a chairman controls bids to talk in a committee meeting, or questions may only be asked by one specific party of another (as in courtroom cross-examination). the unit of conversational communication, expressing a speech act, averaging around 2 s in duration but highly variable; in spoken language, typically a phrase or clause grammatically and prosodically complete and pragmatically sufficient." @default.
- W1903378096 created "2016-06-24" @default.
- W1903378096 creator A5037978750 @default.
- W1903378096 date "2016-01-01" @default.
- W1903378096 modified "2023-10-16" @default.
- W1903378096 title "Turn-taking in Human Communication – Origins and Implications for Language Processing" @default.
- W1903378096 cites W1427377083 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W1533766694 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W1587164474 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W1814449106 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W1916508083 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W1964446421 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W1967820964 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W1968428115 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W1969885740 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W1972436629 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W1977814435 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W1986320816 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W1993055580 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2000196122 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2001292406 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2003837706 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2004022807 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2010352021 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2013056803 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2020483430 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2024160256 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2024985115 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2025064497 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2045012128 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2048944294 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2056260427 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2056949810 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2060971430 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2072921909 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2073165740 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2077530229 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2098212071 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2106272460 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2113016097 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2113355906 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2115969311 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2120762340 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2126055092 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2129090275 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2132686760 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2132951686 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2137066512 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2137374537 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2142227642 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2149781337 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2159941522 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2161345458 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2162634167 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2163343191 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W2989949391 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W316215934 @default.
- W1903378096 cites W4248634141 @default.
- W1903378096 doi "https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.10.010" @default.
- W1903378096 hasPubMedId "https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/26651245" @default.
- W1903378096 hasPublicationYear "2016" @default.
- W1903378096 type Work @default.
- W1903378096 sameAs 1903378096 @default.
- W1903378096 citedByCount "370" @default.
- W1903378096 countsByYear W19033780962016 @default.
- W1903378096 countsByYear W19033780962017 @default.
- W1903378096 countsByYear W19033780962018 @default.
- W1903378096 countsByYear W19033780962019 @default.
- W1903378096 countsByYear W19033780962020 @default.
- W1903378096 countsByYear W19033780962021 @default.
- W1903378096 countsByYear W19033780962022 @default.
- W1903378096 countsByYear W19033780962023 @default.
- W1903378096 crossrefType "journal-article" @default.
- W1903378096 hasAuthorship W1903378096A5037978750 @default.
- W1903378096 hasBestOaLocation W19033780962 @default.
- W1903378096 hasConcept C13280743 @default.
- W1903378096 hasConcept C138885662 @default.
- W1903378096 hasConcept C15744967 @default.
- W1903378096 hasConcept C169760540 @default.
- W1903378096 hasConcept C169900460 @default.
- W1903378096 hasConcept C180747234 @default.
- W1903378096 hasConcept C188147891 @default.
- W1903378096 hasConcept C199360897 @default.
- W1903378096 hasConcept C205649164 @default.
- W1903378096 hasConcept C2776264592 @default.
- W1903378096 hasConcept C2776352735 @default.
- W1903378096 hasConcept C2777200299 @default.
- W1903378096 hasConcept C2777526511 @default.
- W1903378096 hasConcept C41008148 @default.
- W1903378096 hasConcept C41895202 @default.
- W1903378096 hasConcept C46312422 @default.
- W1903378096 hasConcept C511192102 @default.
- W1903378096 hasConcept C89267518 @default.
- W1903378096 hasConceptScore W1903378096C13280743 @default.
- W1903378096 hasConceptScore W1903378096C138885662 @default.
- W1903378096 hasConceptScore W1903378096C15744967 @default.
- W1903378096 hasConceptScore W1903378096C169760540 @default.
- W1903378096 hasConceptScore W1903378096C169900460 @default.