Matches in SemOpenAlex for { <https://semopenalex.org/work/W1972323265> ?p ?o ?g. }
Showing items 1 to 69 of
69
with 100 items per page.
- W1972323265 endingPage "471" @default.
- W1972323265 startingPage "470" @default.
- W1972323265 abstract "In this issue of Neuron, a study by Baumgartner et al. investigates the influence of oxytocin on trust behavior and its neural mechanisms. The authors report that, following breaches of trust, oxytocin facilitates prosocial behavior while modulating neural signals in the amygdala and caudate nucleus. The findings have implications for an array of mental disorders where social behavior is compromised. In this issue of Neuron, a study by Baumgartner et al. investigates the influence of oxytocin on trust behavior and its neural mechanisms. The authors report that, following breaches of trust, oxytocin facilitates prosocial behavior while modulating neural signals in the amygdala and caudate nucleus. The findings have implications for an array of mental disorders where social behavior is compromised. To trust or not to trust is a social dilemma that impacts our way of life. While expressing trust is essential to building social relationships that are important for personal fulfillment and success, decisions to trust can also backfire and result in a lack of reciprocity and eventual resentment toward those that violated trust. Such breaches of confidence can lead to the development of betrayal aversion, potentially influencing how future social interactions are evaluated and trust decisions are executed. In this issue of Neuron, Baumgartner et al., 2008Baumgartner T. Heinrichs M. Vonlanthen A. Fischbacher U. Fehr E. Neuron. 2008; 58 (this issue): 639-650Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (817) Google Scholar attempt to understand the neurobiology underlying trust behavior following a breach in trust by combining pharmacological manipulations with neuroimaging techniques and an economic paradigm called the “trust game”(Camerer and Weigelt, 1988Camerer C. Weigelt K. Econometrica. 1988; 56: 1-36Crossref Google Scholar, Berg et al., 1995Berg J. Dickhaut J. McCabe K. Games Econ. Behav. 1995; 10: 122-142Crossref Scopus (2585) Google Scholar). A typical trust game involves a one-shot social interaction between two players, an investor and a trustee. The investor is faced with a decision to keep a sum of money (e.g., $10) or share it with a trustee. If shared, the investment is tripled ($30) and the trustee now faces the decision to repay the trust by sending back a larger amount of money (e.g., $15 for each participant) or to defect and violate trust by keeping the money, leaving the investor with nothing to show for his display of trust. The social dilemma for the investor is a clear one, as it is potentially more profitable to trust, but doing so leaves the investor susceptible to the risk of a breach in trust. Evidence suggests that humans are traditionally averse to these types of risks (Bohnet and Zeckhauser, 2004Bohnet I. Zeckhauser R. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 2004; 55: 467-484Crossref Scopus (378) Google Scholar), and that this behavior may be modulated by the neuropeptide oxytocin (Kosfeld et al., 2005Kosfeld M. Heinrichs M. Zak P.J. Fischbacher U. Fehr E. Nature. 2005; 435: 673-676Crossref PubMed Scopus (2314) Google Scholar). A hormone released during social touch and childbirth, oxytocin has long been known for its role in social attachment and facilitation of social interactions (Insel and Young, 2001Insel T.R. Young L.J. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2001; 2: 129-136Crossref PubMed Scopus (805) Google Scholar). More recently, intranasal applications of oxytocin have been demonstrated to increase one's tendency to engage in social risks in a trust game, while having no effect on a similar but nonsocial risk game (Kosfeld et al., 2005Kosfeld M. Heinrichs M. Zak P.J. Fischbacher U. Fehr E. Nature. 2005; 435: 673-676Crossref PubMed Scopus (2314) Google Scholar). Oxytocin receptors are abundant in the amygdala (Huber et al., 2005Huber D. Veinante P. Stoop R. Science. 2005; 308: 245-248Crossref PubMed Scopus (601) Google Scholar), a structure involved in emotion and fear learning (Phelps and LeDoux, 2005Phelps E.A. LeDoux J.E. Neuron. 2005; 48: 175-187Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (2064) Google Scholar), and oxytocin administration leads to decreased amygdala blood oxygenated level dependent (BOLD) responses to fearful stimuli (Kirsch et al., 2005Kirsch P. Esslinger C. Chen Q. Mier D. Lis S. Siddhanti S. Gruppe H. Mattay V.S. Gallhofer B. Meyer-Lindenberg A. J. Neurosci. 2005; 25: 11489-11493Crossref PubMed Scopus (1155) Google Scholar). Thus, one interpretation of the Kosfeld et al., 2005Kosfeld M. Heinrichs M. Zak P.J. Fischbacher U. Fehr E. Nature. 2005; 435: 673-676Crossref PubMed Scopus (2314) Google Scholar findings is that oxytocin may aid an individual in overcoming the betrayal aversion that is inherent in such social economic exchanges, increasing the prosocial behavior of sharing. In conjunction with the amygdala, the striatum, particularly the caudate nucleus, a structure involved in reward-related learning and decision-making (Balleine et al., 2007Balleine B.W. Delgado M.R. Hikosaka O. J. Neurosci. 2007; 27: 8161-8165Crossref PubMed Scopus (817) Google Scholar), is also hypothesized to be involved in trust behavior. More specifically, the caudate nucleus has been linked with acquiring reputations or learning to associate a positive outcome (e.g., payoff in trust game) with a particular action (e.g., sharing with trustee X) during repeated iterations of a trust game (King-Casas et al., 2005King-Casas B. Tomlin D. Anen C. Camerer C.F. Quartz S.R. Montague P.R. Science. 2005; 308: 78-83Crossref PubMed Scopus (817) Google Scholar). Failure to take into account the current feedback during such social interactions (which may reflect betrayal) leads to diminished responses in the caudate nucleus and a lack of behavioral adaptation in the trust game (Delgado et al., 2005Delgado M.R. Frank R.H. Phelps E.A. Nat. Neurosci. 2005; 8: 1611-1618Crossref PubMed Scopus (453) Google Scholar). In their study, Baumgartner and colleagues propose that oxytocin reduces betrayal aversion that results from breaches of trust by modulating subcortical targets involved in fear learning and reward-related processing, namely the amygdala and the caudate nucleus (Baumgartner et al., 2008Baumgartner T. Heinrichs M. Vonlanthen A. Fischbacher U. Fehr E. Neuron. 2008; 58 (this issue): 639-650Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (817) Google Scholar). The authors administered oxytocin or placebo to 49 male participants acting as investors in multiple rounds of a trust game (with different trustees) while simultaneously undergoing an fMRI scan. Participants played either a trust game or a risk game in which similar financial risks were taken without a social component (i.e. with a computer). In order to investigate the role of oxytocin following breaches of trust, the experiment was divided into a prefeedback and postfeedback phase. In between the two phases, participants received feedback information indicating that roughly 50% of their decisions (in both trust and risk games) had resulted in poor investments—that is, their trust had been breached (trust game) or their gamble did not pay off (risk game). As expected, participants in the placebo group decreased their expression of trust (measured as amount of money invested) after discovering that their prior displays of trust had been violated; that is, placebo participants shared less in the trust game during the postfeedback phase compared with the prefeedback phase. In contrast, participants that received oxytocin maintained their prosocial behavior of sharing in the trust game, irrespective of breaches of trust. This behavioral difference between placebo and oxytocin group in the postfeedback trust game was marked by neural differences in the hypothesized regions. Compared with the placebo group, the oxytocin group showed less activation in the amygdala and caudate nucleus, in support of the idea that the mechanism by which oxytocin affects social behavior is through a decrease in fear mechanisms associated with betrayal aversion (Kirsch et al., 2005Kirsch P. Esslinger C. Chen Q. Mier D. Lis S. Siddhanti S. Gruppe H. Mattay V.S. Gallhofer B. Meyer-Lindenberg A. J. Neurosci. 2005; 25: 11489-11493Crossref PubMed Scopus (1155) Google Scholar) concurrent with a decrease in immediate feedback processing necessary for guiding future decisions (Delgado et al., 2005Delgado M.R. Frank R.H. Phelps E.A. Nat. Neurosci. 2005; 8: 1611-1618Crossref PubMed Scopus (453) Google Scholar, King-Casas et al., 2005King-Casas B. Tomlin D. Anen C. Camerer C.F. Quartz S.R. Montague P.R. Science. 2005; 308: 78-83Crossref PubMed Scopus (817) Google Scholar). Importantly, these behavioral and neural differences were apparent during the trust game, but not the risk game, further suggesting that the effect of oxytocin is exclusive to social risks. The report by Baumgartner and colleagues represents an ambitious and significant development in the literature, integrating different methodologies (pharmacological and neuroimaging) and disciplines (neuroscience and economics). The study highlights the strength of oxytocin in facilitating social interactions after trust has been violated, by potentially lowering defense mechanisms associated with social risks and by overcoming negative feedback that is important for adapting behavior. While a degree of wariness may protect one from harm, being able to “forgive and forget” is an imperative step in maintaining long-term relationships. This study, therefore, has significant implications for understanding mental disorders where deficits in social behavior are observed. Betrayal aversion, for example, could serve as a precursor to social phobia, a disorder characterized by aversion to social interactions; the reported oxytocin finding could provide a bridge for potential clinical applications. Other questions for future exploration may focus on what constitutes betrayal and how different types of feedback may modulate the stability of the oxytocin finding. Simple manipulations such as different magnitudes (e.g., 80% defection rate), valence (e.g., positive and negative feedback), or even rate of the feedback (e.g., every trial) may or may not influence the documented oxytocin effect. Betrayal, however, may be stronger when preceded by social expectations. A good test of the role of oxytocin in overcoming betrayal aversion, therefore, might involve social interactions where expectations exist, such as a breach in confidence by a loved one. Future investigations may also pursue potential sex differences in interpreting breaches of trust during administration of oxytocin. Trust is essential to building social relationships and breaches of trust have a profound impact on social behavior and mental health. It is worth noting, however, that while oxytocin may facilitate prosocial behavior by potentially reducing betrayal aversion, often times this is not advantageous. As the old proverb states, “Fool me once, shame on you; fool me twice, shame on me.” Oxytocin Shapes the Neural Circuitry of Trust and Trust Adaptation in HumansBaumgartner et al.NeuronMay 22, 2008In BriefTrust and betrayal of trust are ubiquitous in human societies. Recent behavioral evidence shows that the neuropeptide oxytocin increases trust among humans, thus offering a unique chance of gaining a deeper understanding of the neural mechanisms underlying trust and the adaptation to breach of trust. We examined the neural circuitry of trusting behavior by combining the intranasal, double-blind, administration of oxytocin with fMRI. We find that subjects in the oxytocin group show no change in their trusting behavior after they learned that their trust had been breached several times while subjects receiving placebo decrease their trust. Full-Text PDF Open Archive" @default.
- W1972323265 created "2016-06-24" @default.
- W1972323265 creator A5004227153 @default.
- W1972323265 date "2008-05-01" @default.
- W1972323265 modified "2023-09-27" @default.
- W1972323265 title "Fool Me Once, Shame on You; Fool Me Twice, Shame on Oxytocin" @default.
- W1972323265 cites W1998872047 @default.
- W1972323265 cites W2014957672 @default.
- W1972323265 cites W2024365408 @default.
- W1972323265 cites W2045633722 @default.
- W1972323265 cites W2079053008 @default.
- W1972323265 cites W2102027817 @default.
- W1972323265 cites W2117693696 @default.
- W1972323265 cites W2118814351 @default.
- W1972323265 cites W2121748618 @default.
- W1972323265 cites W2138559331 @default.
- W1972323265 cites W2165266294 @default.
- W1972323265 cites W3126017034 @default.
- W1972323265 doi "https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.05.005" @default.
- W1972323265 hasPubMedId "https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/18498730" @default.
- W1972323265 hasPublicationYear "2008" @default.
- W1972323265 type Work @default.
- W1972323265 sameAs 1972323265 @default.
- W1972323265 citedByCount "25" @default.
- W1972323265 countsByYear W19723232652012 @default.
- W1972323265 countsByYear W19723232652015 @default.
- W1972323265 countsByYear W19723232652016 @default.
- W1972323265 countsByYear W19723232652018 @default.
- W1972323265 countsByYear W19723232652019 @default.
- W1972323265 countsByYear W19723232652023 @default.
- W1972323265 crossrefType "journal-article" @default.
- W1972323265 hasAuthorship W1972323265A5004227153 @default.
- W1972323265 hasBestOaLocation W19723232651 @default.
- W1972323265 hasConcept C11171543 @default.
- W1972323265 hasConcept C15744967 @default.
- W1972323265 hasConcept C169760540 @default.
- W1972323265 hasConcept C181612422 @default.
- W1972323265 hasConcept C2776176026 @default.
- W1972323265 hasConcept C2781283010 @default.
- W1972323265 hasConcept C77805123 @default.
- W1972323265 hasConceptScore W1972323265C11171543 @default.
- W1972323265 hasConceptScore W1972323265C15744967 @default.
- W1972323265 hasConceptScore W1972323265C169760540 @default.
- W1972323265 hasConceptScore W1972323265C181612422 @default.
- W1972323265 hasConceptScore W1972323265C2776176026 @default.
- W1972323265 hasConceptScore W1972323265C2781283010 @default.
- W1972323265 hasConceptScore W1972323265C77805123 @default.
- W1972323265 hasIssue "4" @default.
- W1972323265 hasLocation W19723232651 @default.
- W1972323265 hasLocation W19723232652 @default.
- W1972323265 hasOpenAccess W1972323265 @default.
- W1972323265 hasPrimaryLocation W19723232651 @default.
- W1972323265 hasRelatedWork W2748952813 @default.
- W1972323265 hasRelatedWork W2752945631 @default.
- W1972323265 hasRelatedWork W2899084033 @default.
- W1972323265 hasRelatedWork W4210553333 @default.
- W1972323265 hasRelatedWork W4235489569 @default.
- W1972323265 hasRelatedWork W4242119248 @default.
- W1972323265 hasRelatedWork W4254775419 @default.
- W1972323265 hasRelatedWork W4286268198 @default.
- W1972323265 hasRelatedWork W4322716237 @default.
- W1972323265 hasRelatedWork W2132648106 @default.
- W1972323265 hasVolume "58" @default.
- W1972323265 isParatext "false" @default.
- W1972323265 isRetracted "false" @default.
- W1972323265 magId "1972323265" @default.
- W1972323265 workType "article" @default.