Matches in SemOpenAlex for { <https://semopenalex.org/work/W1976621407> ?p ?o ?g. }
Showing items 1 to 78 of
78
with 100 items per page.
- W1976621407 endingPage "524" @default.
- W1976621407 startingPage "523" @default.
- W1976621407 abstract "Behind every analysis of observational data lurks the specter of inadequate control of confounding. Inadequate control can result from inadequate measurement of confounding variables, from errors in measurement or classification of those variables, from incorrect assumptions about the nature of relationships among variables, and from laziness and exhaustion. Analysts faced with hundreds or thousands of measured covariates that potentially confound the association of the study treatment or exposure with the outcome may throw up their hands in desperation; one who tries to use the usual advice for building and checking models will quickly get exhausted. Automating the process of model building thus becomes an appealing option. But how can one automate model building in a way to do a good job of controlling confounding? The problem becomes particularly acute when one is using propensity scores to control confounding; here, modeling the treatment process accurately is not an end in itself, and, in terms of efficiency and even bias, it can be harmful to control for covariates that predict the treatment but not the outcome.1 Automated algorithms have been developed for several purposes: to discover causal structure, to build well-fitting predictive models, and to control for confounding. Algorithms to discover causal structure can, in principle, sometimes determine whether a covariate is affected by treatment and so should not be controlled, or a pretreatment covariate that may need to be controlled.2 Unfortunately, these algorithms may be of limited use in observational epidemiology.3 There is a large literature on automated algorithms for predictive models, both in statistics and machine learning.4 Criteria for evaluating models may relate to measures of predictive ability (eg, R2), or to calibration or discrimination. Many algorithms are designed to try to optimize these measures, and use these measures explicitly in model building and selection. These criteria are based on functions of observable quantities; thus, cross-validation plays a key role, as random subsets of the observed data may be used to evaluate rules derived from other random subsets. Controlling confounding presents a harder problem, because the purpose of the modeling relates to unobservable or counterfactual outcomes.5–7 Thus, evaluation of the performance of a modeling strategy using empirical data will rely on assumptions external to the data; in this way, it is similar to controlling confounding using observational data. The assumptions can relate to the internal data at hand or to external data. Internal assumptions might be that identified measured covariates are sufficient for controlling confounding; this assumption seems implicit in the process used for model and variable selection by Schneeweiss et al8 in this issue of Epidemiology. External assumptions might include an assumption that other studies, especially randomized trials, provide less biased estimates of the same quantities; this also seems to be assumed by Schneeweiss et al. Although there are reasons this might not be true, including differences between the populations used in the randomized studies and the observational studies at hand, it is plausible that this is a good working approximation. Compared with some other approaches for model selection in propensity score models,9 the approach of Schneeweiss et al is presented with little formal or theoretical justification. Nonetheless, on the whole, the algorithm seems reasonable. Formal methods based on minimizing some criterion can be somewhat problematic when using traditional propensity score adjustments with logistic or proportional hazards regression models, because, due to noncollapsibility of the measures of effect, the true parameter of interest changes with the variables included in the propensity score model.9–11 This noncollapsibility may be unimportant if the outcome is rare, as is the case in the examples provided. It is intriguing that in each of the 3 examples, the authors' algorithm produces estimates closer to those expected on the basis of randomized trials or other external knowledge than do simpler adjustment methods using fewer covariates. Although suggestive, this is not fully convincing evidence of the utility of their approach, as the authors acknowledge. These results might be viewed as a meta-study with a sample size of 3; amassing more empirical evidence (by increasing sample size) is clearly a difficult and time-consuming process. Further, there is moderate imprecision in the estimates in each of the studies, and so it is hard to know definitively whether the closer approximation of their results to expectations is due to better performance of their methods in reducing bias. In summary, Schneeweiss et al provide a reasonable-looking algorithm and intriguing if limited evidence for its utility in reducing bias reduction. Despite the difficulty of obtaining empirical evidence of an algorithm's benefit, the use of this algorithm, and other automated algorithms, is to be encouraged. With high-dimensional covariate data, model selection by hand in a reasonable way is an impossible task that is too often attempted in epidemiology. Just as automation was required for the mass production of industrial goods, it will be required for the mass consideration of large numbers of high-dimensional confounders. Schneeweiss et al are to be congratulated for moving our field in this direction. ABOUT THE AUTHOR MARSHALL JOFFE is Associate Professor of Biostatistics at the University of Pennsylvania. His independent research concentrates on statistical methods for causal inference, and he has concentrated on methods for controlling for confounding in general and confounding by indication in particular. He collaborates extensively in nephrology and other fields." @default.
- W1976621407 created "2016-06-24" @default.
- W1976621407 creator A5067046453 @default.
- W1976621407 date "2009-07-01" @default.
- W1976621407 modified "2023-09-23" @default.
- W1976621407 title "Exhaustion, Automation, Theory, and Confounding" @default.
- W1976621407 cites W1480376833 @default.
- W1976621407 cites W1490576214 @default.
- W1976621407 cites W1812519883 @default.
- W1976621407 cites W1967862917 @default.
- W1976621407 cites W1996016456 @default.
- W1976621407 cites W2087373687 @default.
- W1976621407 cites W2132917208 @default.
- W1976621407 cites W2143457654 @default.
- W1976621407 cites W2168458505 @default.
- W1976621407 doi "https://doi.org/10.1097/ede.0b013e3181a82501" @default.
- W1976621407 hasPubMedId "https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/19525688" @default.
- W1976621407 hasPublicationYear "2009" @default.
- W1976621407 type Work @default.
- W1976621407 sameAs 1976621407 @default.
- W1976621407 citedByCount "13" @default.
- W1976621407 countsByYear W19766214072012 @default.
- W1976621407 countsByYear W19766214072015 @default.
- W1976621407 countsByYear W19766214072016 @default.
- W1976621407 countsByYear W19766214072018 @default.
- W1976621407 countsByYear W19766214072020 @default.
- W1976621407 countsByYear W19766214072021 @default.
- W1976621407 countsByYear W19766214072022 @default.
- W1976621407 crossrefType "journal-article" @default.
- W1976621407 hasAuthorship W1976621407A5067046453 @default.
- W1976621407 hasBestOaLocation W19766214071 @default.
- W1976621407 hasConcept C105795698 @default.
- W1976621407 hasConcept C119043178 @default.
- W1976621407 hasConcept C119857082 @default.
- W1976621407 hasConcept C144237770 @default.
- W1976621407 hasConcept C148220186 @default.
- W1976621407 hasConcept C149782125 @default.
- W1976621407 hasConcept C158600405 @default.
- W1976621407 hasConcept C17923572 @default.
- W1976621407 hasConcept C23131810 @default.
- W1976621407 hasConcept C33923547 @default.
- W1976621407 hasConcept C41008148 @default.
- W1976621407 hasConcept C77350462 @default.
- W1976621407 hasConceptScore W1976621407C105795698 @default.
- W1976621407 hasConceptScore W1976621407C119043178 @default.
- W1976621407 hasConceptScore W1976621407C119857082 @default.
- W1976621407 hasConceptScore W1976621407C144237770 @default.
- W1976621407 hasConceptScore W1976621407C148220186 @default.
- W1976621407 hasConceptScore W1976621407C149782125 @default.
- W1976621407 hasConceptScore W1976621407C158600405 @default.
- W1976621407 hasConceptScore W1976621407C17923572 @default.
- W1976621407 hasConceptScore W1976621407C23131810 @default.
- W1976621407 hasConceptScore W1976621407C33923547 @default.
- W1976621407 hasConceptScore W1976621407C41008148 @default.
- W1976621407 hasConceptScore W1976621407C77350462 @default.
- W1976621407 hasIssue "4" @default.
- W1976621407 hasLocation W19766214071 @default.
- W1976621407 hasLocation W19766214072 @default.
- W1976621407 hasLocation W19766214073 @default.
- W1976621407 hasOpenAccess W1976621407 @default.
- W1976621407 hasPrimaryLocation W19766214071 @default.
- W1976621407 hasRelatedWork W2464681697 @default.
- W1976621407 hasRelatedWork W2526929158 @default.
- W1976621407 hasRelatedWork W2540939911 @default.
- W1976621407 hasRelatedWork W2745636525 @default.
- W1976621407 hasRelatedWork W2889343356 @default.
- W1976621407 hasRelatedWork W2982818049 @default.
- W1976621407 hasRelatedWork W3105378735 @default.
- W1976621407 hasRelatedWork W3208489104 @default.
- W1976621407 hasRelatedWork W4286896224 @default.
- W1976621407 hasRelatedWork W4296012301 @default.
- W1976621407 hasVolume "20" @default.
- W1976621407 isParatext "false" @default.
- W1976621407 isRetracted "false" @default.
- W1976621407 magId "1976621407" @default.
- W1976621407 workType "article" @default.