Matches in SemOpenAlex for { <https://semopenalex.org/work/W1996006026> ?p ?o ?g. }
Showing items 1 to 84 of
84
with 100 items per page.
- W1996006026 abstract "The topic editors, Cornelia Herbert and colleagues, have noted that language has historically been assumed to be independent from emotions. The historical backdrop to this is the long reign of faculty psychology, which viewed the human mind as composed of discrete abilities (see discussion in Barrett, 2013). The mental modularity popularized by Chomsky (1965) and Fodor (1983) continued this view following the cognitive revolution of mid-century. Emotion had no role in information processing psychology, leading to its neglect in the cognitive sciences (Cromwell and Panksepp, 2011). Indeed, the classic emotion-cognition divide has been criticized in the past decades by theorists who are otherwise not natural allies (e.g., Damasio, 1994; Cromwell and Panksepp, 2011; Lindquist, 2013). An alternative to faculty psychology is psychological construction (Lindquist, 2013). On this view, mental abilities and mental states like emotions are constructed from the dynamic interaction of physiological states, situation-specific information, and conceptual knowledge.In the modular view of mind, emotion and language should have little overlap in their processes and representations. However, according to psychological constructivism, an emotional reaction can be influenced by any aspect of the on-going situation, such as the language being spoken, which is the topic of this commentary.I describe here findings on the emotionality differences between a native and a foreign language. Bilingual speakers 1 frequently report that swearing, praying, lying, and saying I love you feel differently when using a native rather than a foreign language (see, e.g., Pavlenko, 2005; Dewaele, 2010). My goal is to highlight the relevance of this body of work for the theoretical assumptions regarding language-emotion independence." @default.
- W1996006026 created "2016-06-24" @default.
- W1996006026 creator A5085182910 @default.
- W1996006026 date "2014-09-23" @default.
- W1996006026 modified "2023-10-16" @default.
- W1996006026 title "Emotionality differences between a native and foreign language: theoretical implications" @default.
- W1996006026 cites W1517579943 @default.
- W1996006026 cites W1518634923 @default.
- W1996006026 cites W1976598687 @default.
- W1996006026 cites W1995107738 @default.
- W1996006026 cites W2006373716 @default.
- W1996006026 cites W2006833917 @default.
- W1996006026 cites W2021625970 @default.
- W1996006026 cites W2029645143 @default.
- W1996006026 cites W2039966091 @default.
- W1996006026 cites W2042544886 @default.
- W1996006026 cites W2055273434 @default.
- W1996006026 cites W2060652382 @default.
- W1996006026 cites W2063799857 @default.
- W1996006026 cites W2082385556 @default.
- W1996006026 cites W2097541526 @default.
- W1996006026 cites W2101467160 @default.
- W1996006026 cites W2104159212 @default.
- W1996006026 cites W2107251229 @default.
- W1996006026 cites W2125693923 @default.
- W1996006026 cites W2125927301 @default.
- W1996006026 cites W2135184128 @default.
- W1996006026 cites W2152890993 @default.
- W1996006026 cites W2320323568 @default.
- W1996006026 cites W4234821997 @default.
- W1996006026 cites W4511810 @default.
- W1996006026 doi "https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01055" @default.
- W1996006026 hasPubMedCentralId "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/4172089" @default.
- W1996006026 hasPubMedId "https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/25295019" @default.
- W1996006026 hasPublicationYear "2014" @default.
- W1996006026 type Work @default.
- W1996006026 sameAs 1996006026 @default.
- W1996006026 citedByCount "138" @default.
- W1996006026 countsByYear W19960060262015 @default.
- W1996006026 countsByYear W19960060262016 @default.
- W1996006026 countsByYear W19960060262017 @default.
- W1996006026 countsByYear W19960060262018 @default.
- W1996006026 countsByYear W19960060262019 @default.
- W1996006026 countsByYear W19960060262020 @default.
- W1996006026 countsByYear W19960060262021 @default.
- W1996006026 countsByYear W19960060262022 @default.
- W1996006026 countsByYear W19960060262023 @default.
- W1996006026 crossrefType "journal-article" @default.
- W1996006026 hasAuthorship W1996006026A5085182910 @default.
- W1996006026 hasBestOaLocation W19960060261 @default.
- W1996006026 hasConcept C138496976 @default.
- W1996006026 hasConcept C138885662 @default.
- W1996006026 hasConcept C15744967 @default.
- W1996006026 hasConcept C168862018 @default.
- W1996006026 hasConcept C180747234 @default.
- W1996006026 hasConcept C41895202 @default.
- W1996006026 hasConceptScore W1996006026C138496976 @default.
- W1996006026 hasConceptScore W1996006026C138885662 @default.
- W1996006026 hasConceptScore W1996006026C15744967 @default.
- W1996006026 hasConceptScore W1996006026C168862018 @default.
- W1996006026 hasConceptScore W1996006026C180747234 @default.
- W1996006026 hasConceptScore W1996006026C41895202 @default.
- W1996006026 hasLocation W19960060261 @default.
- W1996006026 hasLocation W19960060262 @default.
- W1996006026 hasLocation W19960060263 @default.
- W1996006026 hasLocation W19960060264 @default.
- W1996006026 hasLocation W19960060265 @default.
- W1996006026 hasOpenAccess W1996006026 @default.
- W1996006026 hasPrimaryLocation W19960060261 @default.
- W1996006026 hasRelatedWork W1975890018 @default.
- W1996006026 hasRelatedWork W1977593659 @default.
- W1996006026 hasRelatedWork W1986831788 @default.
- W1996006026 hasRelatedWork W1995275849 @default.
- W1996006026 hasRelatedWork W2030000401 @default.
- W1996006026 hasRelatedWork W2082505788 @default.
- W1996006026 hasRelatedWork W2144845955 @default.
- W1996006026 hasRelatedWork W2156198431 @default.
- W1996006026 hasRelatedWork W3024157458 @default.
- W1996006026 hasRelatedWork W3137456215 @default.
- W1996006026 hasVolume "5" @default.
- W1996006026 isParatext "false" @default.
- W1996006026 isRetracted "false" @default.
- W1996006026 magId "1996006026" @default.
- W1996006026 workType "article" @default.