Matches in SemOpenAlex for { <https://semopenalex.org/work/W2005198390> ?p ?o ?g. }
- W2005198390 endingPage "465" @default.
- W2005198390 startingPage "457" @default.
- W2005198390 abstract "Free AccessMelatoninInsomnia in Shift Work Disorder Relates to Occupational and Neurophysiological Impairment Ren Belcher, BA, Valentina Gumenyuk, PhD, Thomas Roth, PhD Ren Belcher, BA Sleep Disorders and Research Center, Henry Ford Hospital, Detroit, MI Pritzker School of Medicine, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL Search for more papers by this author , Valentina Gumenyuk, PhD Address correspondence to: Valentina Gumenyuk, PhD, MEG Neuroimaging Center, Meadowlands Hospital 55 Meadowlands Parkway, Secaucus, NJ 07094(201) 392-3058(201) 392-3402 E-mail Address: [email protected] Sleep Disorders and Research Center, Henry Ford Hospital, Detroit, MI Magnetoencephalography Laboratory, Meadowlands Hospital, Secaucus, NJ Search for more papers by this author , Thomas Roth, PhD Sleep Disorders and Research Center, Henry Ford Hospital, Detroit, MI Search for more papers by this author Published Online:April 15, 2015https://doi.org/10.5664/jcsm.4606Cited by:10SectionsAbstractPDF ShareShare onFacebookTwitterLinkedInRedditEmail ToolsAdd to favoritesDownload CitationsTrack Citations AboutABSTRACTStudy Objectives:To determine whether occupational and neurophysiological decrements within shift work disorder (SWD) are differentially related to its two diagnostic symptoms, insomnia and excessive sleepiness.Methods:Thirty-four permanent night workers participated in an overnight lab protocol including a multiple sleep latency test (MSLT) and an event-related brain potential (ERP) task testing auditory target detection (P3a and P3b). At 16:00, each subject completed an Endicott Work Productivity Scale (EWPS), two Insomnia Severity Indices (ISI-Day, ISI-Night), and an Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS). Subjects were grouped by ISI and ESS scores into clinical phenotypes. This study compared EWPS and ERP results between alert insomniacs (“AI,” reporting insomnia without sleepiness), sleepy insomniacs (“SI,” reporting both insomnia and sleepiness), and controls.Results:The AI group was most impaired on the EWPS, significantly more impaired than controls (25.8 ± 14.8 vs. 12.3 ± 9.4, p < 0.05). SI were not statistically different from controls (19.5 ± 8.7 vs. 12.3 ± 9.4, p > 0.05). Compared to controls, AI showed significantly attenuated P3a response (Fcz, Czp, Cpz, mean difference [MD] 1.62–1.77, p < 0.05) and target-detection P3b response (Fcz, Czp, Cpz, MD 1.28–1.64, p < 0.05). P3b in SI was not different from controls (p > 0.10), and P3a was only different at one electrode site (Cpz, MD 1.43, p < 0.01). Neither the MSLT nor the ESS correlated with EWPS scores or ERP (P3a/P3b) amplitudes (p > 0.10). However, the mean of the ISI measurements correlated with the EWPS (r = 0.409, p < 0.01) and the attention-to-novelty P3a (r = −0.410, p < 0.01).Conclusions:Among shift work disorder patients, insomnia is linked to functional and cognitive impairments. Insomniacs with normal sleepiness showed more severe impairments than insomniacs who also reported excessive sleepiness.Citation:Belcher R, Gumenyuk V, Roth T. Insomnia in shift work disorder relates to occupational and neurophysiological impairment. J Clin Sleep Med 2015;11(4):457–465.INTRODUCTIONShift work disorder (SWD) is a circadian rhythm disorder characterized by a chronic mismatch between a shift worker's sleep-wake schedule and his or her circadian clock.1–5 Clinically, this mismatch manifests in insomnia and/or excessive sleepiness. Diagnostic criteria for SWD require the presence of one or both of these symptoms, temporally related to a shift work schedule for at least three months, and not better explained by another medical, psychiatric, or sleep disorder.4Many shift workers meeting diagnostic criteria for SWD report sleep difficulties consistent with those reported by patients with an insomnia disorder, while others report excessive sleepiness (either alone or in combination with insomnia symptoms).3,6 Studies comparing night shift workers with day workers estimate that 44.8% of night workers score greater than 10 on the Epworth Sleepiness Scale (compared to 32.7% of day workers), and 24.7% score greater than 13 (compared to 15.5% of day workers).6 With regard to insomnia, 18.5% of night workers met DSM-IV criteria for the disorder, while only 8.6% of day workers did.6 In conducting previous studies on SWD,1,2,7–9 we have observed that the population of shift workers reporting insomnia can be classified into two phenotypes: those reporting insomnia only, and those reporting insomnia alongside excessive sleepiness. We call the corresponding phenotypes “sleepy insomniacs” (SI) and “alert insomniacs” (AI). The phenotype presenting excessive sleepiness without insomnia (sleepy non-insomniacs [SN]) appears to be less common and is not considered in this study (see Methods).BRIEF SUMMARYCurrent Knowledge/Study Rationale: Workplace accidents, lost productivity, and neurophysiologic impairment are known to be associated with night shift work. This study aimed to determine whether these problems are differently related to insomnia and to excessive sleepiness, two hallmark symptoms that are diagnostic of shift work disorder (SWD) but are not reliably comorbid.Study Impact: Our findings suggest that treatments focusing on excessive sleepiness in SWD may not sufficiently improve work-related outcomes, as overnight occupational and neurophysiologic impairment is more strongly correlated to insomnia than it is to sleepiness. Further research is needed to evaluate the impact of insomnia therapies on workplace safety and productivity in patients with SWD.Functional impairments related to sleep deprivation and/ or circadian misalignment are common in shift work and represent a major productivity and safety risk.10–19 However, most research on performance and safety decrements among shift workers has focused on the deleterious effects of sleepiness,11,20–29 with less regard to shift-work related insomnia. Indeed, most of our knowledge on insomnia and performance is based upon findings in day workers.30–36 Given the different treatment paradigms for excessive sleepiness and insomnia (whether circadian-related or psychophysiologic), investigation into the potentially different occupational sequelae of these two symptoms remains an important priority for research on shift work and SWD. By considering two different clinical presentations of SWD in comparison with healthy night-working controls, this study aims to determine whether occupational and neurophysiological decrements within shift work disorder are differentially related to insomnia and excessive sleepiness.In our previous research,9 we presented evidence of differences in neurophysiological markers, sleepiness, and sleep parameters between the AI and SI phenotypes of SWD. AI patients had increased amplitudes of the N1 ERP, an index of cortical hyperarousal54; normal MSLT during night-working hours; and sleep latency and maintenance problems during both diurnal and nocturnal sleep periods (with no statistically different differences in sleep parameters between the sleep periods: see Table 1). On the other hand, SI patients showed normal N1 amplitudes, suggesting normal cortical arousal and pathological levels of sleepiness during night shift hours.Table 1 Sleep diary, sleepiness, and circadian parameters by SWD phenotype.Table 1 Sleep diary, sleepiness, and circadian parameters by SWD phenotype.These findings led us to evaluate and compare attention-related brain responses (P3a and P3b ERPs), as these provide additional insight into executive dysfunction potentially related to occupational impairment. These neurophysiological measures of brain activity associated with attention and memory are likely affected in people with higher sleep pressure due to either sleep restriction or circadian misalignment. In the current study, conducted within the same sample of night workers as our previous report, we measured the P300 ERP component. This component consists of the P3a and P3b markers, which are associated with involuntary attention switching between target task stimuli and irrelevant distracting stimuli. The P3a has latency around 200–300 ms from deviance onset and a frontocentral scalp distribution. ERP studies in humans with frontal lobe lesions have found that such patients produce a clear diminution of the P3a from a distracting stimulus. Frontal lobe engagement is therefore necessary for P3a generation and mechanisms of attention control.37 Unlike the P3a, the P3b marker is task relevant and associated with target detection. It is elicited when a subject's attention is focused on a stimulus.38 The P3b is associated with the process of memory comparison in the context of the previous stimuli, generating a brain potential with a parietal scalp distribution. The initial processing of a new stimulus engages the switching of attention that underlies P3a production, whereas the subsequent memory comparison engages the operations associated with P3b production. Based on previous research linking wakefulness, level of arousal, and sleep quality to the cognitive P3a and P3b ERP components,39–43 we expected that SWD patients predominantly suffering from insomnia may show different P3a and P3b markers compared to SWD patients who also experience excessive sleepiness.We employed evoked response potentials and the Endicott Work Productivity Scale, a validated measure of work performance,44–46 to assess the functional abilities of night shift workers. The EWPS contains five subscales related to specific attributes of work performance: fatigue, executive function, interpersonal interactions, work efficiency, and counterproductive work behavior. Using groupwise comparisons of these measures between shift workers who present with insomnia, excessive sleepiness, both symptoms, or no symptoms, and using linear regression modeling to compare the symptoms head-to-head, this study aims to test the hypothesis that the two diagnostic symptoms of shift work disorder contribute differentially to occupational functioning and neurophysiology of attention related processes.METHODSThis paper presents a secondary analysis from a larger study. Primary results from the MSLT, sleep diary, circadian phase assessment, and ISI and ESS questionnaires were presented in a previous report9 and are summarized in Table 1.SubjectsPermanent night workers were recruited from healthcare and industrial settings to participate in an overnight laboratory sleep deprivation study. Subjects were required to have worked exclusively on a night shift for ≥ 6 months (≥ 3 night shifts per week, with each shift lasting 8–12 h and occurring between 19:00 and 08:00) and must not have worked a rotating shift or “picked up” daytime shifts during the 6 months preceding the laboratory study. They were required to have low probability for obstructive sleep apnea, restless leg syndrome, narcolepsy, and psychiatric disorders by providing responses in the normal range on the self-completed Berlin Questionnaire and staff-administered Hamilton Depression Scale, as well as in a clinical interview with a sleep medicine physician. Participants who had been diagnosed with insomnia or excessive sleepiness prior to beginning their nighttime work shift were excluded. They were required to be free from head injury, hearing problems, alcohol or substance abuse; must have been nonsmokers; and must not have consumed more than 300 mg of caffeine, on average, per day. Finally, they were required to be free from all CNS-acting medication as well as β-adrenergic blocking agents and other drugs which are known to affect the circadian rhythm, sleep-wake function, or melatonin production. Drugs encountered and disqualified included sertraline, tamoxifen, paroxetine, zolpidem, metoprolol, atenolol, bupropion, alprazolam, venlafaxine, methylphenidate, gabapentin, tramadol, diazepam, triazolam, levocetirizine, buspirone, citalopram, lisdexamfetamine, and trazodone. An exception was made for shift workers with insomnia symptoms who had taken melatonin or sleep-promoting agents while working nighttime shifts, but had subsequently discontinued medication and had been free of the drug and all potential metabolites for ≥ 2 weeks prior to the laboratory study.Ninety-five night workers initially responded to flyers and online newsletter advertisements. Initial advertisements did not specify symptoms of SWD, while subsequent advertisements recruited night workers experiencing “sleepiness or trouble sleeping during the day,” based on recruitment needs. Of the 95 respondents, 35 (36.8%) were disqualified based on an online survey; 13 (13.7%) did not comply with pre-screening requests to keep a sleep diary or failed to keep a screening appointment; 5 (5.3%) were disqualified at the screening appointment; 3 (3.2%) were eligible but not enrolled; and 1 (1.1%) dropped out following enrollment. Thirty-eight subjects (40.0%) qualified based on inclusion criteria. Excluding one subject released for noncompliance, 37 subjects completed the study (mean age 35.41 ± 9.35 years, 62.2% female).At the screening appointment, 26 of 37 qualifying subjects (70.2%) were diagnosed with SWD upon a clinical interview with a sleep medicine physician and review of the participant's screening sleep diary. The remaining 11 (29.7%) were asymptomatic (controls).Self-Reported SleepFor 2 weeks prior to the laboratory study, subjects kept a sleep diary reporting bedtime, wake time, latency to sleep, number of awakenings, use of alcohol and caffeine, and subjective sleep quality.Laboratory ProceduresThe laboratory study was scheduled to begin on an evening after the subject had worked ≥ 3 consecutive night shifts. They were instructed to sleep during the day before coming to the laboratory, as if sleeping before a subsequent night shift. On the study day, they arrived at 16:00 and were housed in a dimly lit (< 15 lux), sound-attenuated private bedroom and bathroom that they would occupy for 25 hours. Participants were aware of clock time. They were not permitted to sleep at any point during their entire experimental session, and were required to remain out of bed except for nap trials during the multiple sleep latency test (see below). Internet access, television, and cellular phone usage were permitted during unscheduled time, and 3 meals were provided at the subject's request in addition to snacks and water. Meals did not contain foods or drinks known to affect sleep or circadian rhythms such as caffeine, ethanol, milk, bananas, or tomatoes. Subjects were also prohibited from consuming crunchy foods that might injure the gums and permit blood contamination of the saliva samples (see below). Registered polysomnographic technologists and research associates continuously monitored all participants for wakefulness throughout the study.QuestionnairesAt 18:00, each subject completed an Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS), 2 separate Insomnia Severity Index (ISI) questionnaires, and the Endicott Work Productivity Scale (EWPS). Items on the separate ISIs were identical, but both verbal and written instructions prompted subjects to separately evaluate their diurnal sleep on one questionnaire (ISI-D) and nocturnal sleep on the other (ISI-N).Phenotype ClassificationTwenty-six subjects with SWD were classified into 3 phenotypes using scores from the ESS and ISI, as well as a diagnostic interview with a sleep medicine physician.9 Twelve subjects presented pathologic scores (≥ 10) on the ISI-D, ISI-N, or both, while scoring in the normal range (< 10) on the ESS. These subjects were classified “alert insomniacs” (AI). Within this subgroup, 11 subjects (91.7%) had pathologic scores on the ISI-D and 8 (66.7%) were flagged for pathologic scores on the ISI-N.Eleven subjects with elevated scores on one or both ISIs as well as the ESS were classified into the sleepy insomniac (SI) group. Within this subgroup, 10 subjects (90.9%) had pathologic scores on the ISI-D, and 4 (36.4%) had pathologic scores on the ISI-N.Eleven controls showed normal scores on the ESS and both ISIs, corroborating the diagnostic interview at the screening appointment.Finally, 3 subjects showed pathologic sleepiness (ESS ≥ 10) but normal scores on both insomnia indices (ISI-D and ISIN < 10). These subjects were classified “sleepy non-insomniacs” (SN). Because of the small number of subjects who met criteria for this group, it was excluded from the analyses.Circadian Phase AssessmentBeginning at 17:00, saliva samples were collected at 30-min intervals using a Salivette tube with a cotton insert (Sarstedt Group, Numbrecht, Germany). Prior to 17:00, subjects were instructed on the procedure of saliva collection and the amount of saliva required for each sample. Each sample was weighed to ensure that ≥ 1 mL of saliva was provided. Saliva was then extracted from the cotton insert in a frozen centrifuge, and samples were frozen at −20°C until being shipped over dry ice to SolidPhase, Inc. (Portland, Maine, USA), where they were radio-immunoassayed. The intra-assay precision was 2.6% to 20.1%, with functional sensitivity of 0.9 pg/mL and analytical sensitivity of 0.2 pg/mL.Dim light melatonin onset (DLMO) is an individual's characteristic point in clock time marking the onset of elevated melatonin levels that normally coincide with the timing of the sleep period.47–49 DLMO was calculated as the time that the amplitude of the fitted LOWESS curve for melatonin concentration rose and remained above a subject's melatonin threshold for at least one hour. The threshold used was the average of the 5 lowest concentrations of melatonin during the 24-h phase assessment, plus 15% of the average of the 5 highest concentrations.Objective Sleepiness AssessmentObjective sleepiness was assessed with a nocturnal multiple sleep latency test (MSLT), following standard research protocol. Electrode integrity was checked by physical inspection and electrical biocalibration before each nap. The first 5 naps (22:30 to 06:30) coincided with night shift hours, while the last 3 (08:30 to 12:30) evaluated the effects of acute sleep deprivation. In this report we are presenting the results of a 4-nap MSLT collected between 22:30 and 04:30, corresponding to night shift hours.P3a and P3b Event Related Brain PotentialThe typical oddball paradigm was used for an active auditory processing task. This paradigm consists of 4 types of regularly occurring sounds as well as a rare auditory target. Seventy percent of the regular sounds were simple tones (duration = 100 ms, frequency = 800 Hz) and served as a standard; 10% of regular sounds were complex tones (environmental sounds), which served as novel stimuli. Not included in the P3a/P3b analyses presented here are deviant frequency simple tones (10%, duration = 100 ms, frequency = 1,000 Hz) and deviant duration simple tones (10%, duration = 150 ms, frequency = 800 Hz). The irregular target was a high-pitch sound presented rarely (25 sounds across 14 min) and interspersed with all other sounds. Participants were instructed to press the button when they heard a target sound by using their dominant index finger. All sounds were presented through earplugs binaurally at a 75 dB SPL (sound pressure level) with 5 ms rise/fall time. All stimuli were presented at a constant inter-stimulus interval of 800 ms. Each session lasted 7.3 min. A total of 2 sessions with a short inter-session break (2 min) were presented to each subject. The clock time of the task was between 18:00 and 19:00 for each subject, a time in the 24-h cycle that occurs at neither the nadir for well-adjusted night workers nor the nadir for unadjusted night workers.1,9 It was thus a “neutral zone” in the circadian cycles of our subjects, and timing the ERP in this window permitted the examination of cognitive differences shortly after waking, without the confounding influence of acute sleep deprivation.EEG Recording and ERP Data AnalysisEEG data was recorded via a 32-channel EEG cap (10-20 system, Easy Cap, Gilching, Germany) and an ASA system (ANT, the Netherlands). Electrooculogram (EOG) was recorded by 2 electrodes at the left and right canthus and 2 electrodes above and below the left eye of the subject. Impedances were kept < 10 kiloΩ, and a band-pass filter was set from 0.1 to 100 Hz. The sampling rate was 1,024 Hz.Data were analyzed off-line using Brain Vision Analyzer software (Brain Products GmbH, Gilching, Germany). ERP data were separately analyzed for each sound type. Each segment began 100 ms prior to the stimulus onset and continued for 700 ms after the stimulus onset. A band-pass filter from 0.1 to 30 Hz was applied to segmented data. Segments in which the EEG or EOG exceeded ± 75 microvolts were excluded from the average. ERPs in response to standard and novel stimuli were averaged separately. Baseline correction (100 ms pre-stimulus interval) was applied to the averaged data. On average, ≥ 300 trials for the standard tone and ≥ 100 trials for the novel sounds were included for each subject.To evaluate ERP data, the obligatory auditory N1 waveform was identified as the largest negative wave at latency 80 to 130 ms from sound onset in each individual grand average corresponding to each type of stimulus (standard and novel). The difference wave between ERPs to novel minus ERPs to standard was computed for each participant and then averaged per group. The peak amplitude and latency of the P3a and P3b of the difference wave were measured within an 200–300 ms and 400–600 ms time window from sound onset at the FCz and Cpz electrodes, as these are typically shown in studies to present the largest amplitude for P3a (FCz, frontal) and P3b (CPz, parietal) ERP components.Statistical AnalysesGroupwise comparisons between strata of shift workers presenting different combinations of symptoms (AI and SI) and presenting no symptoms at all (controls) were performed. These phenotypic differences were analyzed using two-tailed Student's t-tests. Pearson bivariate correlations and linear regression modeling were also used, within the full sample, to clarify the differential contributions of SWD symptoms to variance in EWPS and ERP findings. All statistical comparisons of the P3a and P3b to novel sounds involved computing difference waves (amplitudes in response to novel sounds minus amplitudes to the standard tone). The time windows for mean amplitude comparisons were selected based on the peak amplitude of each brain response at FCz (frontal) and CPz (parietal) electrodes.RESULTSEndicott Work Productivity ScaleNight-working controls presented the lowest scores on the EWPS (12.27 ± 9.41), indicating the lowest level of occupational impairment. Subjects presenting insomnia alongside excessive sleepiness showed higher levels of impairment than controls (19.55 ± 8.69, p = 0.06), and SWD subjects only reporting insomnia had the greatest degree of impairment (25.67 ± 14.84, p < 0.05 vs. controls).Summary data of the EWPS aggregate scores and subscales are presented in Table 2. An interesting and significant difference emerged on the fatigue subscale of the EWPS (items such as losing interest, becoming reckless, and falling asleep at work), with AI subjects reporting higher levels of fatigue than controls (6.3 ± 3.1 vs. 3.4 ± 2.62, p < 0.05 vs. controls). Paradoxically, SI subjects showed lower scores on the fatigue subscale than insomnia-only subjects, not significantly different from controls (4.82 ± 1.72, p > 0.10). This suggests that the EWPS may be sensitive to the distinction between fatigue and sleepiness. Compared to controls, AI subjects also reported higher levels of impairment in work efficiency (items such as working slowly, having to repeat tasks, and daydreaming): AI subjects scored 4.75 ± 3.31 compared to 1.91 ± 2.26 in controls (p < 0.05). SI subjects were not statistically different from controls on any of the EWPS subscales.Table 2 Endicott Work Productivity Scale scores and subscores by SWD phenotype.Table 2 Endicott Work Productivity Scale scores and subscores by SWD phenotype.Event-Related Potentials (P3a and P3b)At the FCz, CZp, and CPz electrodes, alert insomniacs showed significantly attenuated P3a responses (mean difference 1.62–1.77, p < 0.05) and target-detection P3b responses (mean difference 1.28–1.64, p < 0.05) relative to controls (see Figure 1A). P3b in SI was not different from controls (p > 0.10), and P3a was only different at one electrode (CPz), mean difference 1.43, p < 0.01). Figure 1B illustrates the topographical scalp distribution of the P3a and P3b brain responses to the novel sounds.Figure 1 (A) Illustration of ERP difference waveforms corresponding to each group at FCz and CPz electrodes. (B) Topographical scalp distribution of P3a (200–240 ms) and P3b (340–380 ms) activities evaluated in each group. Positive voltage is colored red, negative voltage is colored blue, and white color corresponds to zero. CO, controls; AI, alert insomniacs; SI, sleepy insomniacsDownload FigureCorrelations to Insomnia and Sleepiness MetricsPrimary results from the MSLT, sleep diary, circadian phase assessment, and ISI and ESS questionnaires were presented in a previous report9 and are summarized in Table 1. Neither measure of sleepiness (MSLT or ESS) correlated with EWPS scores or ERP amplitudes (p > 0.10). However, the mean of the 2 ISI measures correlated with the EWPS (r = 0.409, p < 0.01) and the attention-to-novelty P3a (r = −0.410, p < 0.01). This suggests that insomnia in SWD is a strong predictor of occupational and neurophysiological impairment, compared to reports and objective measures of sleepiness.Linear ModelingTable 3 presents the results of 2 linear regression models using the ISI, ESS, and MSLT to predict EWPS scores and ERP amplitudes. The P3a marker at the CPz electrode was the only ERP amplitude impaired in both the AI and SI groups, and was thus selected as the ERP outcome variable for linear modeling. In Models 1a and 1b, the mean of 2 ISIs is the only significant predictor of EWPS score and P3a amplitude. We interpret this as further evidence that insomnia is a stronger predictor of occupational and neurophysiological impairment than sleepiness.Table 3 Linear regression models predicting Endicott Work Productivity Scale scores and P3a amplitude.Table 3 Linear regression models predicting Endicott Work Productivity Scale scores and P3a amplitude.To determine if circadian phase is a factor in the ERP or EWPS assessments, dim-light melatonin onset (DLMO) was added to the other predictive variables in Model 2a and Model 2b. DLMO does not change the significance level or substantially affect the standardized effect size of the ISI on EWPS scores, although it does reduce the significance of the ISI in predicting P3a amplitudes. Results from this analysis confirm that a mean of the 2 ISI assessments is a robust predictor of occupational and neurophysiological functioning, such that higher levels of reported insomnia are linked to higher levels of occupational impairment as well as attenuated (impaired) ERP amplitudes.Relationship between EWPS Scores and ERP AmplitudesTotal EWPS scores were correlated with the P3a, an attention-to-novelty response (Cpz, r = −0.344, p < 0.05). The fatigue subscale was also correlated to the P3a response (CPz, r = −0.523), p < 0.01), as was the executive function subscale (difficulty concentrating, organizing work, and forgetting information; CPz, r = −0.343, p < 0.05). Three subscales measuring interpersonal interactions, work efficiency, and counterproductive work behavior did not significantly relate to the P3a, and none of the EWPS scores related to P3b (target detection) amplitudes. These results suggest that lower EWPS scores in night workers with insomnia are more likely related to the attentional network (P3a) and less related to the memory-updating network (P3b).DISCUSSIONThe primary result of the present study is that insomnia is strongly linked to functional and cognitive impairments in shift workers, while sleepiness appears to make a smaller contribution. Insomniacs without excessive sleepiness showed more severe impairments than insomniacs who report excessive sleepiness. Finally, linear modeling and correlation analyses indicate that the ISI is a more robust predictor of functional sequelae than the MSLT or the ESS.We have previously reported that there are etiological differences between the two insomnia phenotypes seen in SWD.9 It was concluded from this study that alert insomniacs (not suffering from excessive sleepiness) were likely to have a preexisting insomnia vulnerability, which evolved into an insomnia disorder in response to the circadian challenge of shift work. In contrast, the SI phenotype primarily had difficulty sleeping when required to sleep at an inappropriate circadian time (i.e., during the day). This was supported by diary data indicating that SWD patients with insomnia alone have disturbed sleep during both nocturnal and diurnal sleep periods, and ERP data showing an N1 response comparable to that seen in day-working individuals with an insomnia disorder. On the other hand, SWD patients reporting both insomnia and excessive sleepiness slept well on days off (i.e., reverting to nocturnal sleep) and showed normal N1 responses. Importantly, these sleep and ERP differences were not attributable to inter-individual differences in the circadian clock, as both groups showed comparable circadian misalignment (DLMO).The results of the present study extend these findings into the occupational domain and emphasize the real-world stakes of clinical treatment for SWD. First, neurophysiologically, we report that the N1 marker is not the only difference between the two insomnia phenotypes of SWD: the attentional and target-detection markers P3a and P3b are also more impaired in the AI group. Second, we report that these neurophysiological impairmen" @default.
- W2005198390 created "2016-06-24" @default.
- W2005198390 creator A5011832419 @default.
- W2005198390 creator A5061604464 @default.
- W2005198390 creator A5079151440 @default.
- W2005198390 date "2015-04-15" @default.
- W2005198390 modified "2023-10-10" @default.
- W2005198390 title "Insomnia in Shift Work Disorder Relates to Occupational and Neurophysiological Impairment" @default.
- W2005198390 cites W1595027853 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W1610432485 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W1630205487 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W1684235232 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W1808864783 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W1850412549 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W1972482311 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W1988077916 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W1988989711 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W1989645977 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W1993545579 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W1996588118 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W1997250763 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W1998080078 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W2001223278 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W2009762856 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W2013550926 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W2014393713 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W2015122265 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W2020554589 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W2040294444 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W2041316075 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W2042508197 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W2045213705 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W205127463 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W2052555230 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W2053253130 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W2055328551 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W2069122903 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W2069897344 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W2085377943 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W2086213036 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W2087675899 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W2089057387 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W2091773423 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W2100354242 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W2108586971 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W2111203770 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W2129864489 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W2135930253 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W2137422396 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W2140964378 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W2147613748 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W2169823334 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W2318799855 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W2400549344 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W2414780108 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W2415135451 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W2415921503 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W2427904044 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W4254807490 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W51628748 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W75959293 @default.
- W2005198390 cites W89805643 @default.
- W2005198390 doi "https://doi.org/10.5664/jcsm.4606" @default.
- W2005198390 hasPubMedCentralId "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/4365460" @default.
- W2005198390 hasPubMedId "https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/25665690" @default.
- W2005198390 hasPublicationYear "2015" @default.
- W2005198390 type Work @default.
- W2005198390 sameAs 2005198390 @default.
- W2005198390 citedByCount "14" @default.
- W2005198390 countsByYear W20051983902015 @default.
- W2005198390 countsByYear W20051983902016 @default.
- W2005198390 countsByYear W20051983902017 @default.
- W2005198390 countsByYear W20051983902018 @default.
- W2005198390 countsByYear W20051983902019 @default.
- W2005198390 countsByYear W20051983902020 @default.
- W2005198390 countsByYear W20051983902021 @default.
- W2005198390 countsByYear W20051983902022 @default.
- W2005198390 countsByYear W20051983902023 @default.
- W2005198390 crossrefType "journal-article" @default.
- W2005198390 hasAuthorship W2005198390A5011832419 @default.
- W2005198390 hasAuthorship W2005198390A5061604464 @default.
- W2005198390 hasAuthorship W2005198390A5079151440 @default.
- W2005198390 hasBestOaLocation W20051983901 @default.
- W2005198390 hasConcept C111919701 @default.
- W2005198390 hasConcept C118552586 @default.
- W2005198390 hasConcept C152478114 @default.
- W2005198390 hasConcept C1862650 @default.
- W2005198390 hasConcept C2775841894 @default.
- W2005198390 hasConcept C2776176791 @default.
- W2005198390 hasConcept C2781210498 @default.
- W2005198390 hasConcept C41008148 @default.
- W2005198390 hasConcept C548259974 @default.
- W2005198390 hasConcept C70410870 @default.
- W2005198390 hasConcept C71924100 @default.
- W2005198390 hasConcept C99508421 @default.
- W2005198390 hasConceptScore W2005198390C111919701 @default.
- W2005198390 hasConceptScore W2005198390C118552586 @default.
- W2005198390 hasConceptScore W2005198390C152478114 @default.