Matches in SemOpenAlex for { <https://semopenalex.org/work/W2015016650> ?p ?o ?g. }
- W2015016650 endingPage "373" @default.
- W2015016650 startingPage "361" @default.
- W2015016650 abstract "A prominent account of conceptual knowledge proposes that information is distributed over visual, tactile, auditory, motor and verbal-declarative attribute domains to the degree to which these features were activated when the knowledge was acquired [D.A. Allport, Distributed memory, modular subsystems and dysphagia, In: S.K. Newman, R. Epstein (Eds.), Current perspectives in dysphagia, Churchill Livingstone, Edinburgh, 1985, pp. 32-60]. A corollary is that when drawing upon this knowledge (e.g., to answer questions), particular aspects of this distributed information is re-activated as a function of the requirements of the task at hand [L.J. Buxbaum, E.M. Saffran, Knowledge of object manipulation and object function: dissociations in apraxic and non-apraxic subjects. Brain and Language, 82 (2002) 179-199; L.J. Buxbaum, T. Veramonti, M.F. Schwartz, Function and manipulation tool knowledge in apraxia: knowing 'what for' but not 'how', Neurocase, 6 (2000) 83-97; W. Simmons, L. Barsalou, The similarity-in-topography principle: Reconciling theories of conceptual deficits, Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20 (2003) 451-486]. This account predicts that answering questions about object manipulation should activate brain regions previously identified as components of the distributed sensory-motor system involved in object use, whereas answering questions about object function (that is, the purpose that it serves) should activate regions identified as components of the systems supporting verbal-declarative features. These predictions were tested in a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study in which 15 participants viewed picture or word pairs denoting manipulable objects and determined whether the objects are manipulated similarly (M condition) or serve the same function (F condition). Significantly greater and more extensive activations in the left inferior parietal lobe bordering the intraparietal sulcus were seen in the M condition with pictures and, to a lesser degree, words. These findings are consistent with the known role of this region in skilled object use [K.M. Heilman, L.J. Gonzalez Rothi, Apraxia, In: K.M. Heilman, E. Valenstein (Eds.), Clinical Neuropsychology, Oxford University Press, New York, 1993, pp. 141-150] as well as previous fMRI results [M. Kellenbach, M. Brett, K. Patterson, Actions speak louder than functions: the importance of manipulability and action in tool representation, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15 (2003) 30-46] and behavioral findings in brain-lesion patients [L.J. Buxbaum, E.M. Saffran, Knowledge of object manipulation and object function: dissociations in apraxic and non-apraxic subjects, Brain and Language, 82 (2002) 179-199]. No brain regions were significantly more activated in the F than M condition. These data suggest that brain regions specialized for sensory-motor function are a critical component of distributed representations of manipulable objects." @default.
- W2015016650 created "2016-06-24" @default.
- W2015016650 creator A5012820610 @default.
- W2015016650 creator A5023265333 @default.
- W2015016650 creator A5049136249 @default.
- W2015016650 creator A5049854864 @default.
- W2015016650 creator A5058192322 @default.
- W2015016650 creator A5079792922 @default.
- W2015016650 creator A5081807378 @default.
- W2015016650 date "2005-05-01" @default.
- W2015016650 modified "2023-10-02" @default.
- W2015016650 title "Distinctions between manipulation and function knowledge of objects: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging" @default.
- W2015016650 cites W1501651412 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W1821561312 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W1965364790 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W1966014149 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W1970867564 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W1977892431 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W1978154168 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W1982613181 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W1997570186 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W2019370496 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W2036279971 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W2043428207 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W2044539750 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W2044591688 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W2053503719 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W2056124373 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W2058713030 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W2061992576 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W2063237661 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W2077116199 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W2083362717 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W2084885059 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W2094048962 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W2094243598 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W2104926716 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W2105257398 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W2105626347 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W2106741152 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W2107875393 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W2109122618 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W2111337500 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W2131635906 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W2138790588 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W2139376466 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W2144201014 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W2149767656 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W2149981795 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W2152801435 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W2155366833 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W2164002247 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W2164209061 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W2276275738 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W2493265692 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W4231543308 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W4232325718 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W4248385051 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W4253846471 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W4254959286 @default.
- W2015016650 cites W4384107075 @default.
- W2015016650 doi "https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.11.001" @default.
- W2015016650 hasPubMedId "https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/15820643" @default.
- W2015016650 hasPublicationYear "2005" @default.
- W2015016650 type Work @default.
- W2015016650 sameAs 2015016650 @default.
- W2015016650 citedByCount "244" @default.
- W2015016650 countsByYear W20150166502012 @default.
- W2015016650 countsByYear W20150166502013 @default.
- W2015016650 countsByYear W20150166502014 @default.
- W2015016650 countsByYear W20150166502015 @default.
- W2015016650 countsByYear W20150166502016 @default.
- W2015016650 countsByYear W20150166502017 @default.
- W2015016650 countsByYear W20150166502018 @default.
- W2015016650 countsByYear W20150166502019 @default.
- W2015016650 countsByYear W20150166502020 @default.
- W2015016650 countsByYear W20150166502021 @default.
- W2015016650 countsByYear W20150166502022 @default.
- W2015016650 countsByYear W20150166502023 @default.
- W2015016650 crossrefType "journal-article" @default.
- W2015016650 hasAuthorship W2015016650A5012820610 @default.
- W2015016650 hasAuthorship W2015016650A5023265333 @default.
- W2015016650 hasAuthorship W2015016650A5049136249 @default.
- W2015016650 hasAuthorship W2015016650A5049854864 @default.
- W2015016650 hasAuthorship W2015016650A5058192322 @default.
- W2015016650 hasAuthorship W2015016650A5079792922 @default.
- W2015016650 hasAuthorship W2015016650A5081807378 @default.
- W2015016650 hasConcept C14036430 @default.
- W2015016650 hasConcept C154945302 @default.
- W2015016650 hasConcept C15744967 @default.
- W2015016650 hasConcept C169760540 @default.
- W2015016650 hasConcept C169900460 @default.
- W2015016650 hasConcept C180747234 @default.
- W2015016650 hasConcept C188147891 @default.
- W2015016650 hasConcept C2779226451 @default.
- W2015016650 hasConcept C2781238097 @default.
- W2015016650 hasConcept C41008148 @default.
- W2015016650 hasConcept C46312422 @default.