Matches in SemOpenAlex for { <https://semopenalex.org/work/W2016078209> ?p ?o ?g. }
- W2016078209 endingPage "244" @default.
- W2016078209 startingPage "207" @default.
- W2016078209 abstract "The Typology of Voicing and Devoicing W. Leo Wetzels and Joan Mascaró This article provides empirical evidence against the claims that [voice] is a privative feature and that word-internal devoicing can occur in a language without word-final devoicing. The study of voice patterns in a number of languages shows that the feature value [− voice] although it is the unmarked value of the laryngeal feature [voice], can be active phonologically in a fashion parallel to the marked value [+ voice]. Across languages, voice assimilation may occur independently of devoicing and, although it normally affects both [+ voice] and [− voice], it may affect only one value in some languages.* Final devoicing and voicing assimilation are two phenomena that have received broad attention in the literature. In this article we re-examine these phenomena, focusing on the representation of [± voice] and on the proper formulation of the mechanisms responsible for surface (de)voicing effects. Our objective is to argue against a number of assumptions on (de)voicing that underlie recent discussions of these phenomena. One assumption is that there are languages—Yiddish, Serbo-Croatian, and Rumanian are claimed to belong to this class—in which word-final coda consonants constitute exceptions to syllable-final devoicing. We will argue, instead, that these languages have no syllable-final devoicing. We will also show that there is no empirical evidence in favor of the claim that devoicing can affect only part of a cluster of voiced consonants without being prosodically conditioned. It furthermore appears that languages that apply devoicing to a class of segments at the end of a prosodic category n, devoice the same set of segments at the end of all prosodic categories that contain n, i.e. syllable-final devoicing implies word-final devoicing, etc. Most importantly, we will empirically falsify the claim that [− voice] does not belong to the universal set of phonological features by illustrating the assimilation of [− voice] in a variety of languages, postlexically as well as lexically. Our conclusion is that [voice] is a binary feature. 1. A Preliminary typology of voice assimilation and devoicing In some languages voice neutralization (devoicing) occurs at the end of the syllable. In the same language, voice assimilation may or may not occur. Whereas Dutch has voice assimilation, German does not, as is illustrated in Table 1. For the sake of comparison, we add Yiddish,1 which has only assimilation, and Berber, which shows neither devoicing nor generalized assimilation.2 [End Page 207] Click for larger view View full resolution Table 1. A typology of voicing assimilation and devoicing. Standard autosegmental analyses derive surface forms from underlying voiced/voiceless distinctions with rules that change the voicing value through delinking (devoicing) and delinking cum spreading (assimilation). In Table 2 the upper set of structures illustrates the effect of delinking cum spreading, the lower set illustrates delinking in final position (C) and before sonorant consonants [− voc(alic), + son(orant)].3 The representations in the first column of Table 2 show the initial structures, which also correspond to the surface structures in languages like English, which do not show general devoicing or assimilatory effects. In column 2, coda delinking results in devoicing, both in potential assimilatory environments and before sonorants, as in German. If a language has both delinking and spreading, the resulting structures are those in column 3, the Dutch case. Yiddish, which has no delinking in nonassimilatory environments, but where assimilatory environments trigger delinking cum spreading, exemplifies the fourth possibility.4 Click for larger view View full resolution Table 2. Voice effects. [End Page 208] In many languages devoicing and voicing assimilation are not controlled by syllable structure. In such languages devoicing may occur, but is restricted to a word- or phrasefinal (sequence of) obstruent(s). Word-internally before sonorant consonants, a voice contrast is maintained. If assimilation occurs, the last obstruent in a sequence determines the voice value of the complete cluster. The basic typology for non-syllable-final devoicing languages, i.e. types II and IV in Table 1, is shown in Table 3, where Serbo-Croatian is in all relevant respects like Yiddish. Berber again exemplifies a no-effect language.5Table 2. Voice effects. Click for larger view View..." @default.
- W2016078209 created "2016-06-24" @default.
- W2016078209 creator A5036191077 @default.
- W2016078209 creator A5064229097 @default.
- W2016078209 date "2001-01-01" @default.
- W2016078209 modified "2023-10-18" @default.
- W2016078209 title "The Typology of Voicing and Devoicing" @default.
- W2016078209 cites W120842712 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W1484198809 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W1500955912 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W1510877233 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W1521770119 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W1547863965 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W1556783701 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W1572700708 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W1572708723 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W1575528222 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W1586055301 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W1588917007 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W1604323049 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W1606007436 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W1958889295 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W1967517602 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W1970523617 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W1984602578 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W2019220860 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W2022754491 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W2038032085 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W2053778602 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W2069023915 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W2084343433 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W2085472786 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W2086205526 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W2130125679 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W2149454222 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W2159024232 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W2318926071 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W2331978705 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W2896030654 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W289812049 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W3042209115 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W3047640023 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W567683537 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W568790489 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W570333779 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W580325568 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W594145055 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W620150760 @default.
- W2016078209 cites W627590010 @default.
- W2016078209 doi "https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2001.0123" @default.
- W2016078209 hasPublicationYear "2001" @default.
- W2016078209 type Work @default.
- W2016078209 sameAs 2016078209 @default.
- W2016078209 citedByCount "128" @default.
- W2016078209 countsByYear W20160782092012 @default.
- W2016078209 countsByYear W20160782092013 @default.
- W2016078209 countsByYear W20160782092014 @default.
- W2016078209 countsByYear W20160782092015 @default.
- W2016078209 countsByYear W20160782092016 @default.
- W2016078209 countsByYear W20160782092017 @default.
- W2016078209 countsByYear W20160782092018 @default.
- W2016078209 countsByYear W20160782092019 @default.
- W2016078209 countsByYear W20160782092020 @default.
- W2016078209 countsByYear W20160782092021 @default.
- W2016078209 countsByYear W20160782092022 @default.
- W2016078209 crossrefType "journal-article" @default.
- W2016078209 hasAuthorship W2016078209A5036191077 @default.
- W2016078209 hasAuthorship W2016078209A5064229097 @default.
- W2016078209 hasConcept C109089402 @default.
- W2016078209 hasConcept C119857082 @default.
- W2016078209 hasConcept C138885662 @default.
- W2016078209 hasConcept C15744967 @default.
- W2016078209 hasConcept C166957645 @default.
- W2016078209 hasConcept C1701360 @default.
- W2016078209 hasConcept C2776035688 @default.
- W2016078209 hasConcept C2776291640 @default.
- W2016078209 hasConcept C41008148 @default.
- W2016078209 hasConcept C41895202 @default.
- W2016078209 hasConcept C552089266 @default.
- W2016078209 hasConcept C75649859 @default.
- W2016078209 hasConcept C75795011 @default.
- W2016078209 hasConcept C95457728 @default.
- W2016078209 hasConceptScore W2016078209C109089402 @default.
- W2016078209 hasConceptScore W2016078209C119857082 @default.
- W2016078209 hasConceptScore W2016078209C138885662 @default.
- W2016078209 hasConceptScore W2016078209C15744967 @default.
- W2016078209 hasConceptScore W2016078209C166957645 @default.
- W2016078209 hasConceptScore W2016078209C1701360 @default.
- W2016078209 hasConceptScore W2016078209C2776035688 @default.
- W2016078209 hasConceptScore W2016078209C2776291640 @default.
- W2016078209 hasConceptScore W2016078209C41008148 @default.
- W2016078209 hasConceptScore W2016078209C41895202 @default.
- W2016078209 hasConceptScore W2016078209C552089266 @default.
- W2016078209 hasConceptScore W2016078209C75649859 @default.
- W2016078209 hasConceptScore W2016078209C75795011 @default.
- W2016078209 hasConceptScore W2016078209C95457728 @default.
- W2016078209 hasIssue "2" @default.
- W2016078209 hasLocation W20160782091 @default.