Matches in SemOpenAlex for { <https://semopenalex.org/work/W2018283353> ?p ?o ?g. }
Showing items 1 to 75 of
75
with 100 items per page.
- W2018283353 endingPage "R118" @default.
- W2018283353 startingPage "R117" @default.
- W2018283353 abstract "A trade-off between fecundity and life expectancy is fundamental to most theories on the evolution of life histories and ageing. In fact, sexual reproduction has been shown to reduce lifespan in a large number of species across all biological taxa [1Stearns S.C. The Evolution of Life Histories. Oxford University Press, Oxford1992Google Scholar]. The only exception known thus far are eusocial hymenopterans, with reproducing queens usually living much longer than the sterile workers [2Hölldobler B. Wilson E.O. The Ants. Springer Press, Berlin1990Crossref Google Scholar]. Here, we show that in a eusocial mammal, a nearly identical life history has evolved. Among mammals, eusociality has evolved only among the subterranean African mole-rats (Rodentia: Bathyergidae), namely in the naked mole-rat Heterocephalus glaber [3Jarvis J.U.M. Eu-sociality in a mammal – cooperative breeding in naked mole-rat Heterocephalus glaber colonies.Science. 1981; 212: 571-573Crossref PubMed Scopus (309) Google Scholar] and several species of the genus Cryptomys [4Burda H. Individual recognition and incest avoidance in eusocial common mole-rats rather than reproductive suppression by parents.Experientia. 1995; 51: 411-413Crossref PubMed Scopus (81) Google Scholar, 5Bennett N.C. Faulkes C.G. African Mole-Rats: Ecology and Eusociality. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge2000Google Scholar]. Eusociality in mole-rats is characterized by monopolization of reproduction by few individuals (usually a single breeding pair), monogamy of the breeding pair, long-lasting philopatry of the offspring resulting in an overlap of adult generations and cooperative brood care [3Jarvis J.U.M. Eu-sociality in a mammal – cooperative breeding in naked mole-rat Heterocephalus glaber colonies.Science. 1981; 212: 571-573Crossref PubMed Scopus (309) Google Scholar, 4Burda H. Individual recognition and incest avoidance in eusocial common mole-rats rather than reproductive suppression by parents.Experientia. 1995; 51: 411-413Crossref PubMed Scopus (81) Google Scholar, 5Bennett N.C. Faulkes C.G. African Mole-Rats: Ecology and Eusociality. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge2000Google Scholar]. Social mole-rats are known for their remarkable longevity, more than 25 years for naked mole-rats [6Sherman P.W. Jarvis J.U.M. Extraordinary life spans of naked mole-rats (Heterocephalus glaber).J. Zool. 2002; 258: 307-311Crossref Scopus (71) Google Scholar], but a thorough study comparing ageing rates of breeders and non-breeders is still lacking. Using long-term breeding data (1984–2005) of Ansell´s mole-rat Cryptomys anselli, we compared life expectancy of breeding and non-breeding animals held in captivity. Due to strict inbreeding avoidance [4Burda H. Individual recognition and incest avoidance in eusocial common mole-rats rather than reproductive suppression by parents.Experientia. 1995; 51: 411-413Crossref PubMed Scopus (81) Google Scholar], adult offspring remained reproductively quiescent unless removed from their colonies and paired to an unfamiliar mate. This held true also when one or both breeding animals died. Thus, reproductive division of labor was clear-cut in our experimental stock, allowing assignment of each individual to either the ‘breeder’ or ‘non-breeder’ group. Both male and female breeders lived approximately twice as long as non-breeders. No difference was found between the sexes in either reproductive caste. With the exception of a single female, all breeders reached an age of at least 6 years, irrespective of sex. The oldest female breeder was 14.9 years and still alive at the end of data collection. The two oldest male breeders reached ages of nearly 20 years. In contrast, in the non-breeder-group, deaths occurred at a constant rate after reaching maturity, and so far all non-breeders died before their 8th birth date (Figure 1). This pattern was not caused by intrinsic quality differences between breeders and non-breeders which might have resulted from an unintended bias towards high-class-individuals during the selection of breeders. Our pairing scheme was determined by the availability of unfamiliar mates of similar age rather than by any other factor, and neither female nor male future breeders grew faster than non-breeders in their juvenile phase (Figure 2A ). Breeders and non-breeders differ in mean social rank, raising the possibility that social rather than reproductive status causes differential ageing rates. However, in captive Cryptomys anselli colonies there is no competition for mates and barely any for food. As a consequence, aggression levels are very low, and sociopositive behaviours such as allogrooming or huddling, which are shared among all group members, prevail. It is, therefore, unlikely that non-breeders die younger because they suffer from social stress. In support of this assumption, survival probability did not differ between dominant (i.e., early-born) and subdominant (late-born) non-breeders (Figure 2B). We thus conclude that social rank does not affect the pace of ageing in this species. Rate of living can also influence life expectancy. We, therefore, quantified daily activity budgets and found no difference between breeders and non-breeders with regard to the time spent resting, feeding, or working (Figure 2C). Costs of reproduction can be masked by various factors, and positive correlations between individual fertility and longevity have been found in other studies [7Clutton-Brock T.H. Reproductive effort and terminal investment in iteroparous animals.Am. Nat. 1984; 123: 212-229Crossref Scopus (578) Google Scholar, 8Von Holst D. Hutzelmeyer H. Kaetzke P. Khaschei M. Schönheiter R. Socal rank, stress, fitness, and life expectancy in wild rabbits.Naturwissenschaften. 1999; 86: 388-393Crossref PubMed Scopus (67) Google Scholar, 9Tsuji K. Nakata K. Heinze J. Lifespan and Reproduction in a Queenless Ant.Naturwissenschaften. 1996; 83: 577-578Crossref Scopus (52) Google Scholar]. Such results have been interpreted as resulting from either variability in individual quality within the study population [7Clutton-Brock T.H. Reproductive effort and terminal investment in iteroparous animals.Am. Nat. 1984; 123: 212-229Crossref Scopus (578) Google Scholar], social stress suffered by subordinate individuals [8Von Holst D. Hutzelmeyer H. Kaetzke P. Khaschei M. Schönheiter R. Socal rank, stress, fitness, and life expectancy in wild rabbits.Naturwissenschaften. 1999; 86: 388-393Crossref PubMed Scopus (67) Google Scholar], or a more exhaustive life style of non-breeding individuals [9Tsuji K. Nakata K. Heinze J. Lifespan and Reproduction in a Queenless Ant.Naturwissenschaften. 1996; 83: 577-578Crossref Scopus (52) Google Scholar]. In our study, these possibilities can be ruled out, as social rank does not affect life expectancy in Ansell's mole-rats, and breeders do not differ from non-breeders in intrinsic quality or life style besides breeding, yet live approximately twice as long. We conclude, therefore, that sexual activity per se enhances lifespan in this species. This is, to our knowledge, the first time that a survival benefit of reproduction could be shown in a vertebrate. Our results have important implications for understanding the evolution of life histories and ageing. Furthermore, being a long-lived mammal with a bimodal ageing pattern that can be easily induced experimentally, Cryptomys anselli could become a valuable new model organism for studying the proximate mechanisms of ageing. We thank S. Begall, S. Cremers, A. Hartmann, J. Heinze, C. Ingram, L. Keller, M. Moshkin, E. Nevo, A. Schrempf and two anonymous reviewers for constructive comments on earlier versions of the manuscript. Download .pdf (.02 MB) Help with pdf files Document S1. Supplemental Experimental Procedures" @default.
- W2018283353 created "2016-06-24" @default.
- W2018283353 creator A5003879998 @default.
- W2018283353 creator A5036122619 @default.
- W2018283353 date "2006-02-01" @default.
- W2018283353 modified "2023-10-14" @default.
- W2018283353 title "Sexual activity and reproduction delay ageing in a mammal" @default.
- W2018283353 cites W1975862103 @default.
- W2018283353 cites W1995836329 @default.
- W2018283353 cites W2011604748 @default.
- W2018283353 cites W2012574588 @default.
- W2018283353 cites W2054065016 @default.
- W2018283353 cites W2119314098 @default.
- W2018283353 doi "https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2006.02.012" @default.
- W2018283353 hasPubMedId "https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/16488857" @default.
- W2018283353 hasPublicationYear "2006" @default.
- W2018283353 type Work @default.
- W2018283353 sameAs 2018283353 @default.
- W2018283353 citedByCount "84" @default.
- W2018283353 countsByYear W20182833532012 @default.
- W2018283353 countsByYear W20182833532013 @default.
- W2018283353 countsByYear W20182833532014 @default.
- W2018283353 countsByYear W20182833532015 @default.
- W2018283353 countsByYear W20182833532016 @default.
- W2018283353 countsByYear W20182833532017 @default.
- W2018283353 countsByYear W20182833532018 @default.
- W2018283353 countsByYear W20182833532019 @default.
- W2018283353 countsByYear W20182833532020 @default.
- W2018283353 countsByYear W20182833532021 @default.
- W2018283353 countsByYear W20182833532022 @default.
- W2018283353 countsByYear W20182833532023 @default.
- W2018283353 crossrefType "journal-article" @default.
- W2018283353 hasAuthorship W2018283353A5003879998 @default.
- W2018283353 hasAuthorship W2018283353A5036122619 @default.
- W2018283353 hasBestOaLocation W20182833531 @default.
- W2018283353 hasConcept C159575461 @default.
- W2018283353 hasConcept C18903297 @default.
- W2018283353 hasConcept C2778234026 @default.
- W2018283353 hasConcept C500499127 @default.
- W2018283353 hasConcept C54355233 @default.
- W2018283353 hasConcept C59659247 @default.
- W2018283353 hasConcept C78458016 @default.
- W2018283353 hasConcept C86803240 @default.
- W2018283353 hasConcept C90856448 @default.
- W2018283353 hasConceptScore W2018283353C159575461 @default.
- W2018283353 hasConceptScore W2018283353C18903297 @default.
- W2018283353 hasConceptScore W2018283353C2778234026 @default.
- W2018283353 hasConceptScore W2018283353C500499127 @default.
- W2018283353 hasConceptScore W2018283353C54355233 @default.
- W2018283353 hasConceptScore W2018283353C59659247 @default.
- W2018283353 hasConceptScore W2018283353C78458016 @default.
- W2018283353 hasConceptScore W2018283353C86803240 @default.
- W2018283353 hasConceptScore W2018283353C90856448 @default.
- W2018283353 hasIssue "4" @default.
- W2018283353 hasLocation W20182833531 @default.
- W2018283353 hasLocation W20182833532 @default.
- W2018283353 hasOpenAccess W2018283353 @default.
- W2018283353 hasPrimaryLocation W20182833531 @default.
- W2018283353 hasRelatedWork W1992292249 @default.
- W2018283353 hasRelatedWork W2048683981 @default.
- W2018283353 hasRelatedWork W2048865965 @default.
- W2018283353 hasRelatedWork W2064324734 @default.
- W2018283353 hasRelatedWork W2333426424 @default.
- W2018283353 hasRelatedWork W2750280604 @default.
- W2018283353 hasRelatedWork W3003175392 @default.
- W2018283353 hasRelatedWork W4230927181 @default.
- W2018283353 hasRelatedWork W65721319 @default.
- W2018283353 hasRelatedWork W3186961206 @default.
- W2018283353 hasVolume "16" @default.
- W2018283353 isParatext "false" @default.
- W2018283353 isRetracted "false" @default.
- W2018283353 magId "2018283353" @default.
- W2018283353 workType "article" @default.