Matches in SemOpenAlex for { <https://semopenalex.org/work/W2022175656> ?p ?o ?g. }
Showing items 1 to 74 of
74
with 100 items per page.
- W2022175656 endingPage "1268" @default.
- W2022175656 startingPage "1267" @default.
- W2022175656 abstract "EDITORIAL FOCUSStrange Things, Moving Things, Wild Animals. Focus on “Neural Correlates of the Automatic and Goal-Driven Biases in Orienting Spatial Attention”Robert M. McPeekRobert M. McPeekThe Smith-Kettlewell Eye Research Institute, San Francisco, California 94115Published Online:01 Sep 2004https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00408.2004MoreSectionsPDF (14 KB)Download PDF ToolsExport citationAdd to favoritesGet permissionsTrack citations A predator leaps out from behind a rock—that's sure to catch your attention. The idea that certain types of stimuli have a hardwired “hotline” to attention has fascinated psychologists, philosophers, and neuroscientists for more than a century. In 1890, William James enumerated “strange things, moving things, wild animals, bright things, pretty things, metallic things, words, blows, blood” as some of the salient stimuli eliciting what he called “passive immediate sensorial attention.” Indeed, the automatic summoning of attention is important for survival even in modern life as the flash of a turn signal alerts you to the unexpected maneuvers of a sports car that is about to cut you off. However, attention is not held captive by every leaf moving in the wind—or every turn signal flashing on the road. James and his contemporaries understood that the automatic mechanisms guiding attention could be overridden, to an extent, by goal-directed volitional control of attention or “force of will.” This affords the flexibility to pull attention away from the old Cadillac whose turn signal has been blinking, incessantly, since the last Ice Age. In this issue, Fecteau et al. (p. 1728–1737) find fertile ground in exploring the interplay between automatic and goal-driven attention, examining its effects on saccadic reaction times and on visual responses in the superior colliculus (SC).Researchers have long speculated that the spatial-attention and eye-movement systems may share common neural substrates, a view that has received some physiological support recently (e.g., Corbetta et al. 1998; Ignashchenkova et al. 2004; Kustov and Robinson 1996; Moore and Fallah 2001). In the present article, Fecteau et al. investigate the neural correlates of attention in the SC of monkeys performing a variant of Posner's (1980) cueing paradigm. In this classic paradigm, the reaction time required to make a saccadic or manual response to a target is used as a measure of attention at the target location. When a cue is flashed at the future location of the target, attention is automatically drawn, resulting in shorter reaction times to the subsequent target at that location. The initial capture of attention is followed a short time later by a decline in attention, known as the inhibition of return (IOR) (Posner and Cohen 1984), at the cued location. During the past 20 years, these observations have inspired a large body of research examining the time course and conditions under which attentional capture and IOR operate. In recent work, the authors established the presence of a neural correlate of attentional capture and IOR in the visual responses of SC neurons. During attentional capture, target-related activity at the cued location is increased and saccades to the target are triggered earlier (Bell et al. 2004). During IOR, this visual activity is reduced and saccades are delayed (Bell et al. 2004; Dorris et al. 2002). By varying the time between cue and target, the authors map out a detailed picture of the correlation between SC visual activity and the time course of attention as measured by saccade latency. The results would have gratified James, who speculated that “attention . . . will prepare the motor centers, and shorten the work which a stimulus has to perform on them, to produce a given effect when it comes.”The present article examines the extent to which these behavioral and SC correlates of attentional capture and IOR are modulated by goal-driven (volitional) factors. Fecteau et al. address this question within the cueing paradigm by manipulating the informativeness of the cue. They find that the initial visual response to the cue does not depend on its informativeness but that cue informativeness significantly modulates the later cue- and target-related responses. These changes in SC activity correspond closely with changes in saccade latency, confirming that the monkeys are correctly interpreting the informativeness of the cue. The results clearly show that SC visual responses are influenced by both goal-driven and automatic attentional mechanisms. Interestingly, Fecteau et al.'s behavioral and neural measures suggest that goal-driven mechanisms cannot completely override the automatic time course of attention. Such a result was anticipated by early empiricists, as Helmholtz (1867) suggested, “The relation of attention to will . . . is less one of immediate than of mediate control.”These results lead naturally to the question of whether the SC is involved in generating attentional capture and IOR or whether its role is more limited. Some studies have suggested SC involvement in the automatic capture of attention (Ignashchenkova et al. 2004; Kustov and Robinson 1996), and lesion studies in humans have indicated that the SC may also play a role in generating IOR (Posner et al. 1985; Sapir et al. 1999). On the other hand, a study in monkeys suggests that upstream structures are the source of the IOR-related reduction in SC visual activity (Dorris et al. 2002). Under this latter view, SC activity related to IOR can be regarded as a virtual readout of neural activity present across a network of areas involved in coding stimulus salience. Clearly, there is still much to learn, but renewed interest and research along these lines promises finally to answer many of the questions about attention that captivated James and his contemporaries more than a century ago.REFERENCESBell et al. 2004 Bell AH, Fecteau JH, and Munoz DP. Using auditory and visual stimuli to investigate the behavioral and neuronal consequences of reflexive covert orienting. J Neurophysiol 91: 2172–2184, 2004.Link | ISI | Google ScholarCorbetta et al. 1998 Corbetta M, Akbudak E, Conturo TE, Snyder AZ, Ollinger JM, Drury HA, Linenweber MR, Petersen SE, Raichle ME, Van Essen DC, and Shulman GL. A common network of functional areas for attention and eye movements. Neuron 21: 761–773, 1998.Crossref | PubMed | ISI | Google ScholarDorris et al. 2004 Dorris MC, Klein RM, Everling S, and Munoz DP. Contribution of the primate superior colliculus to inhibition of return. J Cog Neurosci 14: 1256–1263.Crossref | PubMed | ISI | Google ScholarFecteau et al. 2004 Fecteau JH, Bell AH, and Munoz DP. Neural correlates of the automatic and goal-driven biases in orienting spatial attention. J Neurophysiol 92: 1728–1737, 2004.Link | ISI | Google ScholarHelmholtz 1890 Helmholtz H. Handbuch der Physiologisehen Optik (Dritte Auflage). Hamburg: Voss, 1867. Translation in: James W. The Principles of Psychology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ. Press, 1890.Google ScholarIgnashchenkova et al. 2004 Ignashchenkova A, Dicke PW, Haarmeier T, and Thier P. Neuron-specific contribution of the superior colliculus to overt and covert shifts of attention. Nat Neurosci 7: 56–64, 2004.Crossref | PubMed | ISI | Google ScholarJames 1890 James W. The Principles of Psychology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ. Press, 1890.Google ScholarKustov and Robinson 1996 Kustov AA and Robinson DL. Shared neural control of attentional shifts and eye movements. Nature 384: 74–77, 1996.Crossref | PubMed | ISI | Google ScholarMoore and Fallah 2001 Moore T and Fallah M. Control of eye movements and spatial attention. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98: 1273–1276, 2001.Crossref | PubMed | ISI | Google ScholarPosner 1980 Posner MI. Orienting of attention. The VIIth Sir Frederic Bartlett Lecture. Q J Exp Psychol 32: 3–25, 1980.Crossref | PubMed | Google ScholarPosner and Cohen 1984 Posner MI and Cohen Y. Components of visual orienting. In: Attention and Performance X, edited by Bouma H and Bouwhuis D. Hillsdale: Erlbaum, 1984, p. 531–556.Google ScholarPosner et al. 1985 Posner MI, Rafal RD, Choate LS, and Vaughan J. Inhibition of return: neural basis and function. Cog Neuropsychol 2: 211–228, 1985.Crossref | ISI | Google ScholarSapir et al. 1999 Sapir A, Soroker N, Berger A, and Henik A. Inhibition of return in spatial attention: Direct evidence for collicular generation. Nat Neurosci 2: 1054–1054, 1999.Google ScholarAUTHOR NOTESAddress for reprint requests and other correspondence: R. M. McPeek, The Smith-Kettlewell Eye Research Institute, 2318 Fillmore St., San Francisco, CA 94115 (E-mail: [email protected]). Download PDF Previous Back to Top Next FiguresReferencesRelatedInformation More from this issue > Volume 92Issue 3September 2004Pages 1267-1268 Copyright & PermissionsCopyright © 2004 by the American Physiological Societyhttps://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00408.2004PubMed15331640History Published online 1 September 2004 Published in print 1 September 2004 Metrics" @default.
- W2022175656 created "2016-06-24" @default.
- W2022175656 creator A5066517426 @default.
- W2022175656 date "2004-09-01" @default.
- W2022175656 modified "2023-09-23" @default.
- W2022175656 title "Strange Things, Moving Things, Wild Animals. Focus on Neural Correlates of the Automatic and Goal-Driven Biases in Orienting Spatial Attention" @default.
- W2022175656 cites W1482686831 @default.
- W2022175656 cites W1502980253 @default.
- W2022175656 cites W1968681922 @default.
- W2022175656 cites W1980548664 @default.
- W2022175656 cites W1985076430 @default.
- W2022175656 cites W1993852355 @default.
- W2022175656 cites W2120273207 @default.
- W2022175656 cites W2121678235 @default.
- W2022175656 cites W2122896223 @default.
- W2022175656 cites W2128146044 @default.
- W2022175656 cites W2137310051 @default.
- W2022175656 cites W2751379301 @default.
- W2022175656 doi "https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00408.2004" @default.
- W2022175656 hasPubMedId "https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/15331640" @default.
- W2022175656 hasPublicationYear "2004" @default.
- W2022175656 type Work @default.
- W2022175656 sameAs 2022175656 @default.
- W2022175656 citedByCount "0" @default.
- W2022175656 crossrefType "journal-article" @default.
- W2022175656 hasAuthorship W2022175656A5066517426 @default.
- W2022175656 hasConcept C120665830 @default.
- W2022175656 hasConcept C121332964 @default.
- W2022175656 hasConcept C129564537 @default.
- W2022175656 hasConcept C15744967 @default.
- W2022175656 hasConcept C169760540 @default.
- W2022175656 hasConcept C169900460 @default.
- W2022175656 hasConcept C180747234 @default.
- W2022175656 hasConcept C188147891 @default.
- W2022175656 hasConcept C192209626 @default.
- W2022175656 hasConcept C2984127161 @default.
- W2022175656 hasConcept C46312422 @default.
- W2022175656 hasConcept C50610925 @default.
- W2022175656 hasConcept C91990242 @default.
- W2022175656 hasConceptScore W2022175656C120665830 @default.
- W2022175656 hasConceptScore W2022175656C121332964 @default.
- W2022175656 hasConceptScore W2022175656C129564537 @default.
- W2022175656 hasConceptScore W2022175656C15744967 @default.
- W2022175656 hasConceptScore W2022175656C169760540 @default.
- W2022175656 hasConceptScore W2022175656C169900460 @default.
- W2022175656 hasConceptScore W2022175656C180747234 @default.
- W2022175656 hasConceptScore W2022175656C188147891 @default.
- W2022175656 hasConceptScore W2022175656C192209626 @default.
- W2022175656 hasConceptScore W2022175656C2984127161 @default.
- W2022175656 hasConceptScore W2022175656C46312422 @default.
- W2022175656 hasConceptScore W2022175656C50610925 @default.
- W2022175656 hasConceptScore W2022175656C91990242 @default.
- W2022175656 hasIssue "3" @default.
- W2022175656 hasLocation W20221756561 @default.
- W2022175656 hasLocation W20221756562 @default.
- W2022175656 hasOpenAccess W2022175656 @default.
- W2022175656 hasPrimaryLocation W20221756561 @default.
- W2022175656 hasRelatedWork W1560581275 @default.
- W2022175656 hasRelatedWork W1719084888 @default.
- W2022175656 hasRelatedWork W2010246592 @default.
- W2022175656 hasRelatedWork W2017185691 @default.
- W2022175656 hasRelatedWork W2059278789 @default.
- W2022175656 hasRelatedWork W2069825244 @default.
- W2022175656 hasRelatedWork W2108225273 @default.
- W2022175656 hasRelatedWork W2725417054 @default.
- W2022175656 hasRelatedWork W2792903782 @default.
- W2022175656 hasRelatedWork W2955753775 @default.
- W2022175656 hasVolume "92" @default.
- W2022175656 isParatext "false" @default.
- W2022175656 isRetracted "false" @default.
- W2022175656 magId "2022175656" @default.
- W2022175656 workType "article" @default.