Matches in SemOpenAlex for { <https://semopenalex.org/work/W2022759268> ?p ?o ?g. }
Showing items 1 to 50 of
50
with 100 items per page.
- W2022759268 endingPage "32" @default.
- W2022759268 startingPage "32" @default.
- W2022759268 abstract "Back to table of contents Previous article Next article Clinical & Research NewsFull AccessAvoidance May Supplant Vigilance in Face of Severe ThreatAaron LevinAaron LevinSearch for more papers by this authorPublished Online:21 May 2010https://doi.org/10.1176/pn.45.10.psychnews_45_10_035AbstractLogically, the worse the stress is, the more wary of danger a person ought to become. But research has shown that the relationship is more complex than that.Experimentally, mild stress produces greater vigilance, but still greater stress produces avoidance, according to a study by American and Israeli researchers.That difference might affect survival and serve as an insight into the factors at work in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), they said.Experimental psychologists have been paying more attention over the last decade to attentional bias—the effect that a threatening word or face on a computer screen has on reaction time.Attentional bias can be measured in the laboratory. The subject sees a face or a word with either threatening or neutral aspects on the screen, then is shown two dots at the same point. The dots may be displayed horizontally or vertically. The person must react to the orientation and press a button with the appropriate response. Normally, if the dots appear after a threatening face or word, the subject has a faster reaction time. Any delay represents avoidance. In theory, a bias toward threat would suggest paying closer attention to the things that could hurt you, but some research indicates that avoidance is common after a threatening cue.“In the last eight years, there has been massive work to understand the neurological and biological basis of this process,” said Yair Bar-Haim, Ph.D., an associate professor of psychology at Tel Aviv University, speaking at the Anxiety Disorders Association of America's annual conference in Baltimore in March. A report on the research will be published in the June American Journal of Psychiatry.Usually, studies of trauma victims ask them to recall the previous traumatic event. So Bar-Haim and colleagues from the National Institute of Mental Health sought more evidence in a very real-world setting, during the conflict in December 2009 and January 2010 between Israel and Hamas militants in Gaza. In this case, the subjects were interviewed during a week when they were under rocket attack.Interviewers gathered data from 131 Israeli civilians living within range of rocket fire from Gaza. The respondents were grouped geographically by warning time available once an alert was sounded. In the zone nearest to Gaza, residents had just 15 seconds to seek shelter. Farther away, they had 30 to 45 seconds, while in the most distant area within range, residents had 60 seconds to find cover. A control group of people living beyond rocket range was also interviewed.The researchers used a battery of tests to evaluate symptoms of PTSD (PTSD Checklist from the National Center for PTSD), depression (Patient Health Questionnaire-9), and state anxiety (State-Trait Anxiety Inventory). They also administered the dot-probe task on laptop computers. Most (123) of the interviews were held in the respondents' apartments.People living within the 15-second alert zone and the 30-45 second zone indicated more PTSD and depression symptoms than did those in the 60-second and no-threat areas, said Bar-Haim.Anxiety was high among those in the 15-second warning area, but was lower, with no significant differences, in the zones farther away.In the attention-bias test, raw reaction times to dot-probe tasks were slower following threatening images than neutral ones among persons with the least warning time.Reaction times of those who had less than 45 seconds to seek shelter were significantly slower than those with at least 60 seconds to find shelter and those outside of rocket range.Furthermore, reaction times and psychopathology were linked, said Bar-Haim. Respondents exposed to life-threatening risks not only exhibited avoidance of, rather than vigilance toward, threat information, they also had more symptoms of PTSD, depression, and anxiety. The delayed reaction times confirm results of existing research suggesting psychomotor slowing in patients with PTSD or depression.The correlation between attention-bias suppression and symptoms may also be a marker for dissociation and avoidance, said Bar-Haim.Dissociation and avoidance only delay the response to imminent danger, placing an individual in greater peril. Also, avoidance is a symptom of PTSD, so understanding its relationship to stress may clarify both etiology and treatment.Future research should prospectively track the associations among stress, symptoms, and attention to threat, Bar-Haim suggested.“Life-Threatening Danger and Suppression of Attention Bias to Threat” is posted at <http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org/cgi/content/abstract/appi.ajp.2009.09070956v1>. ISSUES NewArchived" @default.
- W2022759268 created "2016-06-24" @default.
- W2022759268 creator A5048305459 @default.
- W2022759268 date "2010-05-21" @default.
- W2022759268 modified "2023-09-25" @default.
- W2022759268 title "Avoidance May Supplant Vigilance in Face of Severe Threat" @default.
- W2022759268 doi "https://doi.org/10.1176/pn.45.10.psychnews_45_10_035" @default.
- W2022759268 hasPublicationYear "2010" @default.
- W2022759268 type Work @default.
- W2022759268 sameAs 2022759268 @default.
- W2022759268 citedByCount "0" @default.
- W2022759268 crossrefType "journal-article" @default.
- W2022759268 hasAuthorship W2022759268A5048305459 @default.
- W2022759268 hasConcept C118552586 @default.
- W2022759268 hasConcept C152443987 @default.
- W2022759268 hasConcept C15744967 @default.
- W2022759268 hasConcept C169900460 @default.
- W2022759268 hasConcept C180747234 @default.
- W2022759268 hasConcept C189216375 @default.
- W2022759268 hasConcept C192769605 @default.
- W2022759268 hasConcept C77805123 @default.
- W2022759268 hasConceptScore W2022759268C118552586 @default.
- W2022759268 hasConceptScore W2022759268C152443987 @default.
- W2022759268 hasConceptScore W2022759268C15744967 @default.
- W2022759268 hasConceptScore W2022759268C169900460 @default.
- W2022759268 hasConceptScore W2022759268C180747234 @default.
- W2022759268 hasConceptScore W2022759268C189216375 @default.
- W2022759268 hasConceptScore W2022759268C192769605 @default.
- W2022759268 hasConceptScore W2022759268C77805123 @default.
- W2022759268 hasIssue "10" @default.
- W2022759268 hasLocation W20227592681 @default.
- W2022759268 hasOpenAccess W2022759268 @default.
- W2022759268 hasPrimaryLocation W20227592681 @default.
- W2022759268 hasRelatedWork W1972574439 @default.
- W2022759268 hasRelatedWork W1993669159 @default.
- W2022759268 hasRelatedWork W2054482812 @default.
- W2022759268 hasRelatedWork W2108672095 @default.
- W2022759268 hasRelatedWork W2306143782 @default.
- W2022759268 hasRelatedWork W2360365151 @default.
- W2022759268 hasRelatedWork W2508355556 @default.
- W2022759268 hasRelatedWork W3003950465 @default.
- W2022759268 hasRelatedWork W4210800058 @default.
- W2022759268 hasRelatedWork W4214715443 @default.
- W2022759268 hasVolume "45" @default.
- W2022759268 isParatext "false" @default.
- W2022759268 isRetracted "false" @default.
- W2022759268 magId "2022759268" @default.
- W2022759268 workType "article" @default.