Matches in SemOpenAlex for { <https://semopenalex.org/work/W2026161711> ?p ?o ?g. }
Showing items 1 to 79 of
79
with 100 items per page.
- W2026161711 endingPage "735" @default.
- W2026161711 startingPage "733" @default.
- W2026161711 abstract "In this issue of Neuron, Grossmann et al. provide the first evidence of voice-sensitive regions in the brain of 7-month-old, but not 4-month-old, infants. We discuss the implications of these findings for our understanding of cerebral voice processing in the first months of life. In this issue of Neuron, Grossmann et al. provide the first evidence of voice-sensitive regions in the brain of 7-month-old, but not 4-month-old, infants. We discuss the implications of these findings for our understanding of cerebral voice processing in the first months of life. Vocal communication is at the heart of human life. It relies on speech perception but also on a rich set of abilities to extract, evaluate, and categorize the vast amounts of nonlinguistic information available in voices. These voice cognition abilities allow normal listeners to effortlessly identify a speaker's gender and approximate age, to recognize his/her affective state and whether or not it matches the verbal information, and include more subtle percepts such as attractiveness or trustworthiness that play a key role in our social interactions. Voice cognition abilities appear earlier than speech perception in human development. While phoneme discrimination emerges in babies around 2 months and lexical-semantic processing around 12 to 14 months after birth (Friederici, 2005Friederici A.D. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2005; 9: 481-488Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (100) Google Scholar), infants show already well-developed voice perception abilities. Experiments measuring changes in heart rates in neonates during presentation of different voices demonstrate their ability to discriminate voices and to recognize the voices of their parents (Ockleford et al., 1988Ockleford E.M. Vince M.A. Layton C. Reader M.R. Early Hum. Dev. 1988; 18: 27-36Abstract Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (72) Google Scholar)—an ability even present in fetuses before birth (Kisilevsky et al., 2003Kisilevsky B.S. Hains S.M. Lee K. Xie X. Huang H. Ye H.H. Zhang K. Wang Z. Psychol. Sci. 2003; 14: 220-224Crossref PubMed Scopus (253) Google Scholar). In adults, the cerebral processing of vocal sounds is known to engage “temporal voice areas” (TVA) mostly located along the middle and anterior parts of the superior temporal sulcus (STS; Figure 1). Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies show greater activity in the TVA in response to voices (speech, but also nonspeech vocalizations such as laughs, coughs, etc.) than in response to natural nonvocal sounds (environmental sounds, musical sounds, animal vocalizations, etc.), or amplitude- or frequency-matched acoustical control sounds (Belin et al., 2000Belin P. Zatorre R.J. Lafaille P. Ahad P. Pike B. Nature. 2000; 403: 309-312Crossref PubMed Scopus (1234) Google Scholar, Von Kriegstein and Giraud, 2004Von Kriegstein K. Giraud A.L. Neuroimage. 2004; 22: 948-955Crossref PubMed Scopus (213) Google Scholar). The TVA also show strong sensitivity to affective information in voices (Ethofer et al., 2009Ethofer T. Van De Ville D. Scherer K. Vuilleumier P. Curr. Biol. 2009; 19: 1028-1033Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (169) Google Scholar, Grandjean et al., 2005Grandjean D. Sander D. Pourtois G. Schwartz S. Seghier M.L. Scherer K.R. Vuilleumier P. Nat. Neurosci. 2005; 8: 145-146Crossref PubMed Scopus (330) Google Scholar) and are functionally connected to more anterior regions of the right temporal lobe during speaker recognition (Von Kriegstein and Giraud, 2004Von Kriegstein K. Giraud A.L. Neuroimage. 2004; 22: 948-955Crossref PubMed Scopus (213) Google Scholar). Voice-selective areas were recently observed in the macaque brain (Petkov et al., 2008Petkov C.I. Kayser C. Steudel T. Whittingstall K. Augath M. Logothetis N.K. Nat. Neurosci. 2008; 11: 367-374Crossref PubMed Scopus (245) Google Scholar), indicating a long evolutionary history of cerebral voice processing (Figure 2A ). Comparatively little is know on the appearance of cerebral voice processing along the developmental axis (Figures 2B–2E). The earliest time point currently available on the development of cerebral voice processing is provided by a recent study using electroencephalography (EEG). The contrasts of auditory evoked potentials acquired in adults in response to auditory stimulation with vocal and nonvocal sounds highlights a “fronto-temporal positivity to voice” (FTPV) at around 200 ms after sound onset (Figure 2E; Charest et al., 2009Charest I. Pernet C.R. Rousselet G.A. Quiñones I. Latinus M. Fillion-Bilodeau S. Chartrand J.-P. Belin P. BMC Neurosci. 2009; 10: 127Crossref PubMed Scopus (76) Google Scholar). A robust FTPV has been observed in 4- to 5-year-old children (Figure 2C), suggesting that cerebral voice processing is already well established at age 4 (Rogier et al., 2010Rogier O. Roux S. Belin P. Bonnet-Brilhault F. Bruneau N. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2010; 75: 44-47Crossref PubMed Scopus (26) Google Scholar). But how does cerebral voice processing develop before age 4, at a time when speech perception is not yet operational? In this issue of Neuron, Grossmann et al., 2010Grossmann T. Oberecker R. Koch S.P. Friederici A.D. Neuron. 2010; 65 (this issue): 852-858Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (183) Google Scholar bring an important missing piece to the puzzle. They used near infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) to examine cerebral voice processing in 4- and 7-month-old infants. NIRS quantifies the way cerebral tissues absorb different wavelengths of light to extract information on blood oxygenation, indirectly related to neuronal activity. The noninvasive nature of this technique is well adapted to investigations in infants. In a first experiment, Grossman et al. (2010) examined whether voice-sensitive cerebral activity similar to that observed in adults could be evidenced. They contrasted measures of blood oxygenation acquired during stimulation with brief vocal or nonvocal stimuli taken from the “voice localizer” scan routinely used in fMRI studies of adult voice processing (Figure 1). Seven-month-old infants showed greater responses to voices versus other sounds in the posterior part of the temporal lobe bilaterally, with greater voice-sensitive response in the right hemisphere, while no brain region was found exhibiting the opposite pattern. In the 4-month-old infants, however, no such voice-sensitive response was observed. A second experiment then examined the effects of affective information. Measures of blood oxygenation were acquired in 7-month-old infants during stimulation with affectively neutral sentences and sentences spoken with happy or angry emotional prosody. Two regions showed a significant modulation of their activity by affective content, both located in the right hemisphere: a region of posterior temporal cortex shown to be voice sensitive in experiment 1 and a region of the inferior prefrontal cortex, showing small responses but significantly greater for happy vocalizations. These results provide the first available evidence of cerebral voice sensitivity in 7-month-old infants. Cerebral voice processing thus not only has a long evolutionary history (Petkov et al., 2008Petkov C.I. Kayser C. Steudel T. Whittingstall K. Augath M. Logothetis N.K. Nat. Neurosci. 2008; 11: 367-374Crossref PubMed Scopus (245) Google Scholar, Petkov et al., 2009Petkov C.I. Logothetis N.K. Obleser J. Neuroscientist. 2009; 15: 419-429Crossref PubMed Scopus (47) Google Scholar), it also develops relatively early in infants (Figure 2), long before speech perception is fully developed (Friederici, 2005Friederici A.D. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2005; 9: 481-488Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (100) Google Scholar). The pattern of voice sensitivity observed in infants by Grossmann et al., 2010Grossmann T. Oberecker R. Koch S.P. Friederici A.D. Neuron. 2010; 65 (this issue): 852-858Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (183) Google Scholar is comparable to the one observed in adult subjects. Yet the location of voice-sensitive cortex in infants appears more posterior than the one observed in adult (typically along middle/anterior STS, although the posterior temporal lobe posterior also exhibits significant voice sensitivity), suggesting that more anterior regions are not yet voice-sensitive 7 months after birth. Second, they provide a precise timeline for the emergence of cerebral voice processing in infants. Measures of cerebral activity in 4-month-old infants do not show any evidence of regions responding more to sounds of voice compared to nonvocal sounds; in fact, they even showed the reverse pattern in one region, a rare occurrence in the adult brain. Only 3 months later, cerebral voice sensitivity has emerged. This highlights the period between 4 and 7 months after birth as critical for the development of cerebral voice processing. Third, the findings highlight the crucial importance of the right hemisphere in the development of vocal communication in infants. The difference in voice sensitivity in the 7-month-old infants was much greater in the right than in the left hemisphere, as was the modulation by affective content. This indicates a central role of the “minor” right hemisphere, too often neglected, in the emergence of vocal communication ability in human infants. In adults, the TVA are not only sensitive to voices, but also highly selective: they respond more to voice stimuli than to a range of control sounds, including acoustically matched control sounds, showing that the TVA's preference for voice stimuli is not simply driven by low-level acoustical parameters (Belin et al., 2000Belin P. Zatorre R.J. Lafaille P. Ahad P. Pike B. Nature. 2000; 403: 309-312Crossref PubMed Scopus (1234) Google Scholar). Grossmann et al., 2010Grossmann T. Oberecker R. Koch S.P. Friederici A.D. Neuron. 2010; 65 (this issue): 852-858Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (183) Google Scholar demonstrate clear voice sensitivity in the 7-month-old brain; however, it is possible that this activity reflects the processing of low-level features more present in vocal sounds rather than voice processing per se. This is particularly the case for experiment 2, in which the greater response to affective (happy and angry) compared to neutral voices could reflect the processing of the many acoustical differences between these sound categories (e.g., f0 range) without implying emotional processing. Thus, it is important that follow-up experiments test the influence of acoustical structure by using additional control sound categories. During the first months after birth, synaptic proliferation continues at a high rate, associated with an exponential increase in metabolic activity during the first postnatal months (Chugani and Phelps, 1986Chugani H.T. Phelps M.E. Science. 1986; 231: 840-843Crossref PubMed Scopus (509) Google Scholar). Myelination is also taking place at the same time. The auditory radiation that innervates Heschl's gyrus (containing the primary auditory cortex) shows apparent myelination at around 6 months of age (Nakagawa et al., 1998Nakagawa H. Iwasaki S. Kichikawa K. Fukusumi A. Taoka T. Ohishi H. Uchida H. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol. 1998; 19: 1129-1136PubMed Google Scholar). Thus, between 4 and 7 months, the region of the temporal lobe where the TVA are located in adults is undergoing rapid structural changes, which may underlie the acquisition of specialization for voice processing. The precise time course of those structural changes in the STS still needs to be investigated more precisely. In parallel, behavioral studies of voice perception ability in this first year of life—not just speech perception, increasingly well understood (Friederici, 2005Friederici A.D. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2005; 9: 481-488Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (100) Google Scholar), but also, more relevant for infants, nonverbal voice perception—are required to fully understand how functional changes observed by Grossmann et al., 2010Grossmann T. Oberecker R. Koch S.P. Friederici A.D. Neuron. 2010; 65 (this issue): 852-858Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (183) Google Scholar translate at the behavioral level. The fact that adults with autism sometimes show abnormal activation of the TVA (Gervais et al., 2004Gervais H. Belin P. Boddaert N. Leboyer M. Coez A. Sfaello I. Barthélémy C. Brunelle F. Samson Y. Zilbovicius M. Nat. Neurosci. 2004; 7: 801-802Crossref PubMed Scopus (335) Google Scholar) suggests that children with disorders affecting vocal communication, such as autism, could also be characterized by anomalies of cerebral voice processing, that—one may speculate—could potentially be observable at an early stage of development. Grossmann et al., 2010Grossmann T. Oberecker R. Koch S.P. Friederici A.D. Neuron. 2010; 65 (this issue): 852-858Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (183) Google Scholar present results averaged across a group of 16 to 18 infants; whether or not NIRS can provide robust results at the individual level remains unclear. Nevertheless, the present findings open the important and exciting possibility of tools for assessing cerebral voice processing in infants and contributing to the early diagnosis and treatment of voice communication impairments in children. The Developmental Origins of Voice Processing in the Human BrainGrossmann et al.NeuronMarch 25, 2010In BriefIn human adults, voices are processed in specialized brain regions in superior temporal cortices. We examined the development of this cortical organization during infancy by using near-infrared spectroscopy. In experiment 1, 7-month-olds but not 4-month-olds showed increased responses in left and right superior temporal cortex to the human voice when compared to nonvocal sounds, suggesting that voice-sensitive brain systems emerge between 4 and 7 months of age. In experiment 2, 7-month-old infants listened to words spoken with neutral, happy, or angry prosody. Full-Text PDF Open Access" @default.
- W2026161711 created "2016-06-24" @default.
- W2026161711 creator A5044453435 @default.
- W2026161711 creator A5047363391 @default.
- W2026161711 date "2010-03-01" @default.
- W2026161711 modified "2023-10-17" @default.
- W2026161711 title "Before Speech: Cerebral Voice Processing in Infants" @default.
- W2026161711 cites W1497204367 @default.
- W2026161711 cites W1968048563 @default.
- W2026161711 cites W2007475263 @default.
- W2026161711 cites W2017161938 @default.
- W2026161711 cites W2021107445 @default.
- W2026161711 cites W2024797122 @default.
- W2026161711 cites W2030271375 @default.
- W2026161711 cites W2047240413 @default.
- W2026161711 cites W2054951946 @default.
- W2026161711 cites W2084411436 @default.
- W2026161711 cites W2084747841 @default.
- W2026161711 cites W2133238304 @default.
- W2026161711 cites W2136754809 @default.
- W2026161711 cites W2155187681 @default.
- W2026161711 doi "https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.018" @default.
- W2026161711 hasPubMedId "https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/20346748" @default.
- W2026161711 hasPublicationYear "2010" @default.
- W2026161711 type Work @default.
- W2026161711 sameAs 2026161711 @default.
- W2026161711 citedByCount "42" @default.
- W2026161711 countsByYear W20261617112012 @default.
- W2026161711 countsByYear W20261617112013 @default.
- W2026161711 countsByYear W20261617112014 @default.
- W2026161711 countsByYear W20261617112015 @default.
- W2026161711 countsByYear W20261617112016 @default.
- W2026161711 countsByYear W20261617112017 @default.
- W2026161711 countsByYear W20261617112018 @default.
- W2026161711 countsByYear W20261617112019 @default.
- W2026161711 countsByYear W20261617112020 @default.
- W2026161711 countsByYear W20261617112021 @default.
- W2026161711 countsByYear W20261617112022 @default.
- W2026161711 countsByYear W20261617112023 @default.
- W2026161711 crossrefType "journal-article" @default.
- W2026161711 hasAuthorship W2026161711A5044453435 @default.
- W2026161711 hasAuthorship W2026161711A5047363391 @default.
- W2026161711 hasBestOaLocation W20261617111 @default.
- W2026161711 hasConcept C15744967 @default.
- W2026161711 hasConcept C169760540 @default.
- W2026161711 hasConcept C28490314 @default.
- W2026161711 hasConcept C41008148 @default.
- W2026161711 hasConcept C46312422 @default.
- W2026161711 hasConcept C548259974 @default.
- W2026161711 hasConcept C71924100 @default.
- W2026161711 hasConceptScore W2026161711C15744967 @default.
- W2026161711 hasConceptScore W2026161711C169760540 @default.
- W2026161711 hasConceptScore W2026161711C28490314 @default.
- W2026161711 hasConceptScore W2026161711C41008148 @default.
- W2026161711 hasConceptScore W2026161711C46312422 @default.
- W2026161711 hasConceptScore W2026161711C548259974 @default.
- W2026161711 hasConceptScore W2026161711C71924100 @default.
- W2026161711 hasIssue "6" @default.
- W2026161711 hasLocation W20261617111 @default.
- W2026161711 hasLocation W20261617112 @default.
- W2026161711 hasOpenAccess W2026161711 @default.
- W2026161711 hasPrimaryLocation W20261617111 @default.
- W2026161711 hasRelatedWork W1485446539 @default.
- W2026161711 hasRelatedWork W1719619818 @default.
- W2026161711 hasRelatedWork W2007721624 @default.
- W2026161711 hasRelatedWork W2100959543 @default.
- W2026161711 hasRelatedWork W2127703805 @default.
- W2026161711 hasRelatedWork W2748952813 @default.
- W2026161711 hasRelatedWork W2765975811 @default.
- W2026161711 hasRelatedWork W2899084033 @default.
- W2026161711 hasRelatedWork W2931662336 @default.
- W2026161711 hasRelatedWork W3007667582 @default.
- W2026161711 hasVolume "65" @default.
- W2026161711 isParatext "false" @default.
- W2026161711 isRetracted "false" @default.
- W2026161711 magId "2026161711" @default.
- W2026161711 workType "article" @default.