Matches in SemOpenAlex for { <https://semopenalex.org/work/W2028146265> ?p ?o ?g. }
Showing items 1 to 89 of
89
with 100 items per page.
- W2028146265 endingPage "1930" @default.
- W2028146265 startingPage "1929" @default.
- W2028146265 abstract "Modern electronic gaming machines (EGMs), including slot machines, are regarded as among the more high-risk forms of gambling (e.g. 1), but given the array of structural characteristics that these games offer, isolating where any ‘addictiveness’ might come from is a significant challenge for gambling researchers. The new paper by Dixon and colleagues focuses on ‘Losses disguised as wins’ (LDWs) in multi-line slot machines 2. LDWs occur when a player wins a smaller amount than their initial wager. While these events are accompanied by the traditional ‘bells and whistles’ of a jackpot win, they in fact constitute an overall loss. The first experimental study of this effect used psychophysiology to show that LDWs in the commercial game ‘Lobstermania’ increased arousal to a similar extent as true wins 3. Subsequent work has confirmed that players experiencing LDWs overestimate the number of true wins in a session 4. The latest paper develops this story in two important ways. First, this game feature is linked to a behavioural measure, the ‘post-reinforcement pause’ (PRP), which is a delay in the latency to initiate an instrumental response (i.e. the spin press) following a consummatory reward. Their participants played 250 spins on a multi-line game (betting 1 credit each on the maximum 20 lines) and 250 spins on an equivalent single-line game (betting 1 credit on a single line). The games were exceptionally realistic simulations, and the order of the two games was counterbalanced across participants. On both games, participants showed a PRP to the true wins compared to the 0-credit outcomes. Critically, a gain of 2 credits constituted a true win for the single-line group but an LDW for the multi-line, but across both groups the clear step in the PRP latencies occurred from 0 to 2 credits. In fact, the PRP data show exquisite sensitivity to the outcome magnitude (see also 5) and this is valuable proof-of-concept data. PRPs can also be elicited in experimental animals on operant schedules, paving the way for translational work looking at structural characteristics in rodent models of gambling 6. The second important advance is the demonstration of these effects in regular and problem gamblers. Twenty-two of 102 gamblers were classified as high-risk/problem gamblers. In the overall group, these regular slots players overwhelmingly preferred the multi-line game and overestimated the win frequency in the multi-line game, corroborating key effects from the prior studies in university students. The high-risk/problem gamblers found the multi-line game more absorbing and felt more skilful at the game, compared to the single-line game. These data provide provocative links to the recent hypothesis by Dow-Schull 7 that EGMs may be particularly potent at inducing the ‘machine zone’, or psychological flow, and the wider literature on cognitive distortions, including the illusion of control. While these results link the absorption and competency effects in problem gamblers to multi-line slot machines, their data fall short of the more tempting link to the LDWs as a specific game feature. In order to equate their two conditions as best as possible, the multi-line game demanded higher bets and offered higher jackpots than the single-line game, as well as delivering LDWs. This serves to highlight the profound difficulties in isolating specific structural characteristics in gambling games. Disambiguating these effects can require somewhat contrived experimental manipulations, which can then be criticized for lacking ecological validity. We also note that the regular gamblers recruited by Dixon et al. were mainly seniors with a mean age of 61 years, which is considerably older than the peak age of gambling involvement and problem gambling onset. Older adults are classically regarded as risk-averse 8, but may also be more susceptible to the cognitive distortions that occur during gambling 9. From reading this elegant series of papers by Dixon and colleagues, one might conclude erroneously that the LDWs were somehow specific to multi-line slot machines. This is not the case. The most common form of gambling in a British sample of treatment-seeking pathological gamblers was electronic roulette played on Fixed Odds Betting Terminals (FOBTs) 10. These machines also offer an array of bets that can be placed simultaneously (e.g. colour, number) creating a high frequency of LDWs, which can also occur even in non-electronic forms, such as a ‘Yankee’ or a ‘Canadian’ accumulator in sports betting. Studying form in sports betting, or previous number runs in roulette, can instil a feeling of skill or predictive control which may be reinforced by LDWs. We also recognize that the LDW is a gambling event and not a discrete cognitive process in itself. The bells and whistles of winning constitute powerful Pavlovian conditioned stimuli that will certainly contribute to their mechanism, but LDWs may also draw upon additional effects from psychology, such as the ‘framing’ effect 11, that a net loss is flipped into a ‘gain frame’. The Centre for Gambling Research at UBC is supported by an award from the British Columbia Lottery Corporation and the British Columbia Government. L.C. acknowledges funding from the Medical Research Council (G1100554). S.S. was funded by a Cambridge Home and European Scholarship Scheme (CHESS) graduate scholarship from the University of Cambridge." @default.
- W2028146265 created "2016-06-24" @default.
- W2028146265 creator A5003784928 @default.
- W2028146265 creator A5021225992 @default.
- W2028146265 date "2014-10-08" @default.
- W2028146265 modified "2023-10-10" @default.
- W2028146265 title "Commentary on <scp>D</scp>ixon <i>et al</i>. (2014): Understanding the abuse liability of modern electronic gaming machines" @default.
- W2028146265 cites W1514134191 @default.
- W2028146265 cites W1968462069 @default.
- W2028146265 cites W1973481178 @default.
- W2028146265 cites W1999807821 @default.
- W2028146265 cites W2031848389 @default.
- W2028146265 cites W2063416921 @default.
- W2028146265 cites W2076648407 @default.
- W2028146265 cites W2091469126 @default.
- W2028146265 cites W2096452841 @default.
- W2028146265 cites W2113024478 @default.
- W2028146265 doi "https://doi.org/10.1111/add.12697" @default.
- W2028146265 hasPubMedId "https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/25297962" @default.
- W2028146265 hasPublicationYear "2014" @default.
- W2028146265 type Work @default.
- W2028146265 sameAs 2028146265 @default.
- W2028146265 citedByCount "4" @default.
- W2028146265 countsByYear W20281462652015 @default.
- W2028146265 countsByYear W20281462652017 @default.
- W2028146265 countsByYear W20281462652018 @default.
- W2028146265 countsByYear W20281462652022 @default.
- W2028146265 crossrefType "journal-article" @default.
- W2028146265 hasAuthorship W2028146265A5003784928 @default.
- W2028146265 hasAuthorship W2028146265A5021225992 @default.
- W2028146265 hasBestOaLocation W20281462651 @default.
- W2028146265 hasConcept C112698675 @default.
- W2028146265 hasConcept C144133560 @default.
- W2028146265 hasConcept C154945302 @default.
- W2028146265 hasConcept C15744967 @default.
- W2028146265 hasConcept C17744445 @default.
- W2028146265 hasConcept C198352243 @default.
- W2028146265 hasConcept C199539241 @default.
- W2028146265 hasConcept C2524010 @default.
- W2028146265 hasConcept C2723826 @default.
- W2028146265 hasConcept C2777834853 @default.
- W2028146265 hasConcept C2779182362 @default.
- W2028146265 hasConcept C2779295839 @default.
- W2028146265 hasConcept C33923547 @default.
- W2028146265 hasConcept C41008148 @default.
- W2028146265 hasConcept C76155785 @default.
- W2028146265 hasConcept C77805123 @default.
- W2028146265 hasConcept C82876162 @default.
- W2028146265 hasConceptScore W2028146265C112698675 @default.
- W2028146265 hasConceptScore W2028146265C144133560 @default.
- W2028146265 hasConceptScore W2028146265C154945302 @default.
- W2028146265 hasConceptScore W2028146265C15744967 @default.
- W2028146265 hasConceptScore W2028146265C17744445 @default.
- W2028146265 hasConceptScore W2028146265C198352243 @default.
- W2028146265 hasConceptScore W2028146265C199539241 @default.
- W2028146265 hasConceptScore W2028146265C2524010 @default.
- W2028146265 hasConceptScore W2028146265C2723826 @default.
- W2028146265 hasConceptScore W2028146265C2777834853 @default.
- W2028146265 hasConceptScore W2028146265C2779182362 @default.
- W2028146265 hasConceptScore W2028146265C2779295839 @default.
- W2028146265 hasConceptScore W2028146265C33923547 @default.
- W2028146265 hasConceptScore W2028146265C41008148 @default.
- W2028146265 hasConceptScore W2028146265C76155785 @default.
- W2028146265 hasConceptScore W2028146265C77805123 @default.
- W2028146265 hasConceptScore W2028146265C82876162 @default.
- W2028146265 hasFunder F4320334626 @default.
- W2028146265 hasIssue "11" @default.
- W2028146265 hasLocation W20281462651 @default.
- W2028146265 hasLocation W20281462652 @default.
- W2028146265 hasLocation W20281462653 @default.
- W2028146265 hasOpenAccess W2028146265 @default.
- W2028146265 hasPrimaryLocation W20281462651 @default.
- W2028146265 hasRelatedWork W3012257603 @default.
- W2028146265 hasRelatedWork W3016450995 @default.
- W2028146265 hasRelatedWork W3138244192 @default.
- W2028146265 hasRelatedWork W3177475962 @default.
- W2028146265 hasRelatedWork W4230197055 @default.
- W2028146265 hasRelatedWork W4239368213 @default.
- W2028146265 hasRelatedWork W4292264782 @default.
- W2028146265 hasRelatedWork W4296749040 @default.
- W2028146265 hasRelatedWork W621808327 @default.
- W2028146265 hasRelatedWork W644007644 @default.
- W2028146265 hasVolume "109" @default.
- W2028146265 isParatext "false" @default.
- W2028146265 isRetracted "false" @default.
- W2028146265 magId "2028146265" @default.
- W2028146265 workType "article" @default.