Matches in SemOpenAlex for { <https://semopenalex.org/work/W2038768818> ?p ?o ?g. }
Showing items 1 to 76 of
76
with 100 items per page.
- W2038768818 endingPage "R1005" @default.
- W2038768818 startingPage "R1004" @default.
- W2038768818 abstract "Chimpanzee memory has been extensively studied [1Menzel E.W. Chimpanzee spatial memory organization.Science. 1973; 182: 943-945Crossref PubMed Scopus (295) Google Scholar, 2Fujita K. Matsuzawa T. Delayed figure reconstruction by a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and humans (Homo sapiens).J. Comp. Psychol. 1990; 104: 345-351Crossref Google Scholar]. The general assumption is that, as with many other cognitive functions, it is inferior to that of humans [3Premack D. Human and animal cognition: Continuity and discontinuity.Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2007; 104: 13861-13867Crossref PubMed Scopus (184) Google Scholar]; some data, however, suggest that, in some circumstances, chimpanzee memory may indeed be superior to human memory [4Tinklepaugh O.L. The multiple delayed reaction with chimpanzees and monkeys.J. Comp. Psychol. 1932; 13: 207-243Crossref Scopus (104) Google Scholar]. Here we report that young chimpanzees have an extraordinary working memory capability for numerical recollection — better even than that of human adults tested in the same apparatus following the same procedure. Chimpanzee memory has been extensively studied [1Menzel E.W. Chimpanzee spatial memory organization.Science. 1973; 182: 943-945Crossref PubMed Scopus (295) Google Scholar, 2Fujita K. Matsuzawa T. Delayed figure reconstruction by a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and humans (Homo sapiens).J. Comp. Psychol. 1990; 104: 345-351Crossref Google Scholar]. The general assumption is that, as with many other cognitive functions, it is inferior to that of humans [3Premack D. Human and animal cognition: Continuity and discontinuity.Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2007; 104: 13861-13867Crossref PubMed Scopus (184) Google Scholar]; some data, however, suggest that, in some circumstances, chimpanzee memory may indeed be superior to human memory [4Tinklepaugh O.L. The multiple delayed reaction with chimpanzees and monkeys.J. Comp. Psychol. 1932; 13: 207-243Crossref Scopus (104) Google Scholar]. Here we report that young chimpanzees have an extraordinary working memory capability for numerical recollection — better even than that of human adults tested in the same apparatus following the same procedure. Our subjects were six chimpanzees, three mother–offspring pairs. One of the mothers, Ai, was the first chimpanzee who learned to use Arabic numerals to label sets of real-life objects with the corresponding number [5Matsuzawa T. Use of numbers by a chimpanzee.Nature. 1985; 315: 57-59Crossref PubMed Scopus (269) Google Scholar]. The other five chimpanzees had also participated in many previous studies [6Matsuzawa T. Tomonaga M. Tanaka M. Cognitive Development in Chimpanzees. Springer, Tokyo2006Crossref Scopus (8) Google Scholar], but they were naïve to tasks employing numerals. In 2004, when the three young reached the age of four years, we began to teach the mother–offspring pairs the sequence of Arabic numerals from 1 to 9, using a touch-screen monitor connected to a computer. In the numerical sequence task, each trial was unique, in which the nine numerals appeared in different on-screen positions. Accurate performance with 1–2–3–4–5–6–7–8–9 spontaneously transferred to non-adjacent sequences such as 2–3–5–8–9. All naïve chimpanzees successfully learned this numerical sequence task (See Video clip 1 in the Supplemental data available on-line with this issue). A ‘masking task’ to test memory was introduced at around the time when the young became five years old. In this task, after touching the first numeral, all other numerals were replaced by white squares. The subject had to remember which numeral appeared in which location, and then touch them based on the knowledge of numerical sequence. All five naïve chimpanzees mastered the masking task, just like Ai [7Kawai N. Matsuzawa T. Numeric memory span in a chimpanzee.Nature. 2000; 403: 39-40Crossref PubMed Scopus (125) Google Scholar]. It must be noted that the chance level of this task is very low: p= 1/24 with four numerals, 1/120 with five numerals, and so on, down to 1/362,880 with nine numerals. In general, the performance of the three young chimpanzees was better than that of the three mothers (see Table S1 in the Supplemental data). Ayumu, Ai's son, was the best performer among the subjects (Figure 1 and Video clip 2). Humans were slower than all of the three young chimpanzees in the response (Figure S1 in the Supplemental data; Video clips 3 and 4). We developed a new test called the ‘limited-hold memory task’ as a novel way of comparing the working memory of chimpanzee and human subjects. In this task, after the touch to the initial white circle, the numerals appeared only for a certain limited duration, and were then automatically replaced by white squares. Three different hold duration conditions were tested: 650, 430 and 210 milliseconds. The duration of 650 milliseconds was equivalent to the average initial latency of five-numeral masking task. The shortest duration, 210 milliseconds, is close to the frequency of occurrence of human saccadic eye movement [8Bartz A.E. Eye-movement latency, duration, and response time as a function of angular displacement.J. Exp. Psychol. 1962; 64: 318-324Crossref PubMed Scopus (73) Google Scholar]. This means that this condition does not leave subjects enough time to explore the screen by eye movement. The limited-hold memory task provided a means of performing an objective comparison between the two species under exactly identical conditions. We compared Ai, the best mother performer, Ayumu, the best young performer, and human subjects (n = 9, all university students) in this task. Figure 2 shows the results of the comparison between two chimpanzees and human subjects in the limited-hold memory task. The number of numerals was limited to five items. For example, the numerals 2, 3, 5, 8, 9 might appear very briefly on the screen and then be replaced by white squares. Subjects had to touch the squares in the correct order indicated by the original numerals. In human subjects, the percentage of correct trials decreased as a function of the hold duration: the shorter the duration became, the worse the accuracy was (Video clip 5). Ai's performance was below that of the human subjects' average, and showed the same tendency to worsen with shorter duration. From the very first session, however, Ayumu's performance remained at almost the same level, regardless of the hold duration, showing no decline comparable to that of the other subjects (Video clip 6, and Figure S2 in the Supplemental data). These data showed that the chimpanzee subjects can memorize at a glance the Arabic numerals scattered on the touch screen monitor and Ayumu outperformed all of the human subjects both in speed and accuracy. Our results may be reminiscent of the phenomenon known as ‘eidetic imagery’ found by Jaensch [9Jaensch E.R. trans. by Oscar OeserEidetic Imagery and Typological Methods of Investigation.Second Edition. Harcourt, Brace and Co, New York1930Google Scholar, 10Conway A. Jarrold C. Kane M. Miyake A. Towse J. Variation in Working Memory. Oxford University Press, New York2007Google Scholar]. Eidetic imagery has been defined as the memory capability to retain an accurate, detailed image of a complex scene or pattern. It is known to be present in a relatively high percentage of normal children, and then the ability declines with age. Our present study shows that young chimpanzees can quickly grasp many numerals at a glance, with no decline in performance as the hold duration is varied. Moreover, the young ones showed better performance than adults in the memory task. Our study shows that young chimpanzees have an extraordinary working memory capability for numerical recollection better than that of human adults. The results fit well with what we know about the eidetic imagery in humans. This study was supported by grants MEXT16002001, JSPS-GCOE (A06, Biodiversity), and JSPS-HOPE to TM. This is a part of PhD thesis of SI. Thanks are due to Sumiharu Nagumo for his help with computer programming, Masaki Tomonaga, Masayuki Tanaka, Misato Hayashi, Etsuko Nogami and Tomoko Takashima for their support, and Dora Biro for her comments and revisions to the manuscript. Thanks are also due to the veterinary staffs and caregivers of the KUPRI chimpanzees. Download .pdf (.03 MB) Help with pdf files Document S1. Supplemental Methods, Supplemental Experimental Procedures, Supplemental Results, Two Figures, and One Table Download .mpg (3.49 MB) Help with mpg files Movie S1 Download .mpg (4.04 MB) Help with mpg files Movie S2 Download .mpg (2.81 MB) Help with mpg files Movie S3 Download .mpg (3.39 MB) Help with mpg files Movie S4 Download .mpg (3.88 MB) Help with mpg files Movie S5 Download .mpg (3.86 MB) Help with mpg files Movie S6 Download .mpg (4.0 MB) Help with mpg files Movie S7" @default.
- W2038768818 created "2016-06-24" @default.
- W2038768818 creator A5042480969 @default.
- W2038768818 creator A5048233868 @default.
- W2038768818 date "2007-12-01" @default.
- W2038768818 modified "2023-10-16" @default.
- W2038768818 title "Working memory of numerals in chimpanzees" @default.
- W2038768818 cites W1988040501 @default.
- W2038768818 cites W1994626457 @default.
- W2038768818 cites W2029428261 @default.
- W2038768818 cites W2029988244 @default.
- W2038768818 cites W2036336513 @default.
- W2038768818 cites W2037559111 @default.
- W2038768818 cites W2083743019 @default.
- W2038768818 doi "https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2007.10.027" @default.
- W2038768818 hasPubMedId "https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/18054758" @default.
- W2038768818 hasPublicationYear "2007" @default.
- W2038768818 type Work @default.
- W2038768818 sameAs 2038768818 @default.
- W2038768818 citedByCount "275" @default.
- W2038768818 countsByYear W20387688182012 @default.
- W2038768818 countsByYear W20387688182013 @default.
- W2038768818 countsByYear W20387688182014 @default.
- W2038768818 countsByYear W20387688182015 @default.
- W2038768818 countsByYear W20387688182016 @default.
- W2038768818 countsByYear W20387688182017 @default.
- W2038768818 countsByYear W20387688182018 @default.
- W2038768818 countsByYear W20387688182019 @default.
- W2038768818 countsByYear W20387688182020 @default.
- W2038768818 countsByYear W20387688182021 @default.
- W2038768818 countsByYear W20387688182022 @default.
- W2038768818 countsByYear W20387688182023 @default.
- W2038768818 crossrefType "journal-article" @default.
- W2038768818 hasAuthorship W2038768818A5042480969 @default.
- W2038768818 hasAuthorship W2038768818A5048233868 @default.
- W2038768818 hasBestOaLocation W20387688181 @default.
- W2038768818 hasConcept C154945302 @default.
- W2038768818 hasConcept C15744967 @default.
- W2038768818 hasConcept C169760540 @default.
- W2038768818 hasConcept C180747234 @default.
- W2038768818 hasConcept C188147891 @default.
- W2038768818 hasConcept C204160518 @default.
- W2038768818 hasConcept C41008148 @default.
- W2038768818 hasConcept C78458016 @default.
- W2038768818 hasConcept C86803240 @default.
- W2038768818 hasConceptScore W2038768818C154945302 @default.
- W2038768818 hasConceptScore W2038768818C15744967 @default.
- W2038768818 hasConceptScore W2038768818C169760540 @default.
- W2038768818 hasConceptScore W2038768818C180747234 @default.
- W2038768818 hasConceptScore W2038768818C188147891 @default.
- W2038768818 hasConceptScore W2038768818C204160518 @default.
- W2038768818 hasConceptScore W2038768818C41008148 @default.
- W2038768818 hasConceptScore W2038768818C78458016 @default.
- W2038768818 hasConceptScore W2038768818C86803240 @default.
- W2038768818 hasIssue "23" @default.
- W2038768818 hasLocation W20387688181 @default.
- W2038768818 hasLocation W20387688182 @default.
- W2038768818 hasOpenAccess W2038768818 @default.
- W2038768818 hasPrimaryLocation W20387688181 @default.
- W2038768818 hasRelatedWork W1828955125 @default.
- W2038768818 hasRelatedWork W1997770566 @default.
- W2038768818 hasRelatedWork W2034736453 @default.
- W2038768818 hasRelatedWork W2044499740 @default.
- W2038768818 hasRelatedWork W2061542922 @default.
- W2038768818 hasRelatedWork W2096678084 @default.
- W2038768818 hasRelatedWork W2190176143 @default.
- W2038768818 hasRelatedWork W2319374022 @default.
- W2038768818 hasRelatedWork W3048727301 @default.
- W2038768818 hasRelatedWork W3193780050 @default.
- W2038768818 hasVolume "17" @default.
- W2038768818 isParatext "false" @default.
- W2038768818 isRetracted "false" @default.
- W2038768818 magId "2038768818" @default.
- W2038768818 workType "article" @default.