Matches in SemOpenAlex for { <https://semopenalex.org/work/W2080172709> ?p ?o ?g. }
Showing items 1 to 75 of
75
with 100 items per page.
- W2080172709 endingPage "437" @default.
- W2080172709 startingPage "435" @default.
- W2080172709 abstract "The Demise of the Caboose . . . and the Need for Word Consciousness Peter V. Paul, Editor The title of this essay may be confusing, especially because there is a conflation of two different concepts. Let’s start by blaming Cheri Williams for this situation, given her invited essay in this issue of the Annals on vocabulary learning in young children. As you might have surmised, vocabulary is a construct that causes me to become reflective, rational, creative, and unpredictably chaotic. (Full disclosure: My doctoral dissertation was on this topic [Paul, 1984], as well as a few subsequent publications.) As aptly stated by Stahl and Nagy (2006), “Words are so pervasive in our life, so central to being human, that we do not often stop to reflect on their value and power” (p. 3). So many words, so little time. Nevertheless, the goal, in my view, should be the development and enhancement of word consciousness in children and adolescents (see also Blachowicz & Fisher, 2004; Scott & Nagy, 2004; Stahl & Nagy, 2006). What is word consciousness? Briefly, this is a deep and wide awareness of words and their connections to other words. This awareness leads to the understanding of numerous concepts, mainly because vocabulary knowledge is cumulative. The more words and related aspects you know, the more words you will learn in a variety of situations. In addition, you develop this insatiable need for wordplay. To illustrate a few multiple aspects of word consciousness, I shall tell you a story. My son and I get our kicks by going for long car rides, and, invariably, we have to drive across railroad tracks, held together by numerous rows of wood ties. Occasionally, the long crossing gates lower themselves slowly, and the red lights start to blink alternately and incessantly at us. Here comes the freight train, led by a huge, black, robust engine, followed by a few bland-looking gondola cars filled with freight, a boxcar with wide sliding doors, and on and on—but, by golly, there is no cute-looking little, red, yellow, blue, or whatever caboose. As the gates lift themselves up, I look at my son in the rearview mirror, and I inquire: What happened to the caboose? Why do they permit a gondola car to bring up the rear? Where is the crew eating? If you travel by ship, you need a galley for preparing and eating meals. Doesn’t this train need a caboose for that reason and more? Don’t we all like to see a nice-looking caboose that has windows with a view? Hey, let’s go kick the conductor and the engineer in the caboose! By the way, what is more than one caboose? Cabeese like geese? Or caboose like moose? Perhaps, it should be cabooses like boys, to play it safe. By now, my son is either sleeping, daydreaming, or simply not motivated to dialogue with me. Unfortunately, so are, I fear, a number of students in my university classes. Oh, my preservice student-teachers have been exposed to and understand a variety of classic fun-and-interesting research-based vocabulary instruction techniques that fall under the categorical umbrella of semantic elaboration, such as word maps, semantic maps, graphic organizers, semantic feature analyses, webs, four-square concept maps, Venn diagrams, and other depictions of the conceptual fields of words and their relations to other words and phrases (e.g., Gunning, 2008; Heimlich & Pittelman, 1986; Johnson & Pearson, 1984; Stahl & Nagy, 2006). They seem to understand the big three and their combinations for developing vocabulary learning in children—also discussed in Dr. Williams’s essay: (a) incidental word learning via rich language exposure from through-the-air dialogues, discussions, storytellings, story sharings, read-alouds, and so on; (b) extensive reading and re-reading of a wide range of texts and other materials; and (c) explicit, targeted vocabulary instruction by teachers (see also the discussions in Cain & Oakhill, 2007; Carlisle & Rice, 2002; Kamhi & Catts, 2012; Stahl & Fairbanks, 1986). So what’s the problem? Well, to promote word consciousness (e.g., Blachowicz & Fisher, 2004; Scott & Nagy, 2004; Stahl & Nagy, 2006), one must be curious, passionate, and downright nutty about the uses and applications of [End Page 435] words..." @default.
- W2080172709 created "2016-06-24" @default.
- W2080172709 creator A5087761177 @default.
- W2080172709 date "2012-01-01" @default.
- W2080172709 modified "2023-10-16" @default.
- W2080172709 title "The Demise of the Caboose . . . and the Need for Word Consciousness" @default.
- W2080172709 cites W1542246834 @default.
- W2080172709 cites W1560681490 @default.
- W2080172709 cites W1569737621 @default.
- W2080172709 cites W1604443973 @default.
- W2080172709 cites W1894616663 @default.
- W2080172709 cites W2078732706 @default.
- W2080172709 cites W2114811706 @default.
- W2080172709 cites W3216593914 @default.
- W2080172709 cites W645598426 @default.
- W2080172709 doi "https://doi.org/10.1353/aad.2012.1598" @default.
- W2080172709 hasPubMedId "https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/22524089" @default.
- W2080172709 hasPublicationYear "2012" @default.
- W2080172709 type Work @default.
- W2080172709 sameAs 2080172709 @default.
- W2080172709 citedByCount "2" @default.
- W2080172709 countsByYear W20801727092012 @default.
- W2080172709 countsByYear W20801727092015 @default.
- W2080172709 crossrefType "journal-article" @default.
- W2080172709 hasAuthorship W2080172709A5087761177 @default.
- W2080172709 hasConcept C130440534 @default.
- W2080172709 hasConcept C136197465 @default.
- W2080172709 hasConcept C138885662 @default.
- W2080172709 hasConcept C154945302 @default.
- W2080172709 hasConcept C15744967 @default.
- W2080172709 hasConcept C169760540 @default.
- W2080172709 hasConcept C17744445 @default.
- W2080172709 hasConcept C186720457 @default.
- W2080172709 hasConcept C188147891 @default.
- W2080172709 hasConcept C199539241 @default.
- W2080172709 hasConcept C2777601683 @default.
- W2080172709 hasConcept C2777720223 @default.
- W2080172709 hasConcept C41008148 @default.
- W2080172709 hasConcept C41895202 @default.
- W2080172709 hasConceptScore W2080172709C130440534 @default.
- W2080172709 hasConceptScore W2080172709C136197465 @default.
- W2080172709 hasConceptScore W2080172709C138885662 @default.
- W2080172709 hasConceptScore W2080172709C154945302 @default.
- W2080172709 hasConceptScore W2080172709C15744967 @default.
- W2080172709 hasConceptScore W2080172709C169760540 @default.
- W2080172709 hasConceptScore W2080172709C17744445 @default.
- W2080172709 hasConceptScore W2080172709C186720457 @default.
- W2080172709 hasConceptScore W2080172709C188147891 @default.
- W2080172709 hasConceptScore W2080172709C199539241 @default.
- W2080172709 hasConceptScore W2080172709C2777601683 @default.
- W2080172709 hasConceptScore W2080172709C2777720223 @default.
- W2080172709 hasConceptScore W2080172709C41008148 @default.
- W2080172709 hasConceptScore W2080172709C41895202 @default.
- W2080172709 hasIssue "5" @default.
- W2080172709 hasLocation W20801727091 @default.
- W2080172709 hasLocation W20801727092 @default.
- W2080172709 hasOpenAccess W2080172709 @default.
- W2080172709 hasPrimaryLocation W20801727091 @default.
- W2080172709 hasRelatedWork W2028391526 @default.
- W2080172709 hasRelatedWork W2040987357 @default.
- W2080172709 hasRelatedWork W2144608574 @default.
- W2080172709 hasRelatedWork W2178439137 @default.
- W2080172709 hasRelatedWork W2482313458 @default.
- W2080172709 hasRelatedWork W2504006257 @default.
- W2080172709 hasRelatedWork W3022589904 @default.
- W2080172709 hasRelatedWork W3116889747 @default.
- W2080172709 hasRelatedWork W3164075730 @default.
- W2080172709 hasRelatedWork W4280601978 @default.
- W2080172709 hasVolume "156" @default.
- W2080172709 isParatext "false" @default.
- W2080172709 isRetracted "false" @default.
- W2080172709 magId "2080172709" @default.
- W2080172709 workType "article" @default.