Matches in SemOpenAlex for { <https://semopenalex.org/work/W2081490497> ?p ?o ?g. }
Showing items 1 to 71 of
71
with 100 items per page.
- W2081490497 endingPage "R808" @default.
- W2081490497 startingPage "R806" @default.
- W2081490497 abstract "A recent study has shown that Jacky lizards adjust their movement-based visual signaling in response to the varying environmental conditions; the results indicate that this species has highly sophisticated communication and sensory processing strategies. A recent study has shown that Jacky lizards adjust their movement-based visual signaling in response to the varying environmental conditions; the results indicate that this species has highly sophisticated communication and sensory processing strategies. Communication usually involves the targeted exchange of information between a sender and a receiver, using a mutually agreed code. Evolution has shaped a rich spectrum of communication systems amongst animals, exploiting a striking range of channels for transmitting information [1Bradbury J.W. Vehrencamp S.L. Principles of Animal Communication. Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, MA1998Google Scholar]. We find cases of third parties tuning into signal exchanges to eavesdrop, as well as cases in which misleading signals are sent to unsuspecting receivers — camouflage, encryption and code-breaking are some of the more thrilling ‘information management’ strategies that are used not just by humans. Communication across species boundaries, however, is comparatively rare and rather limited in scope, usually characterised by one-directional information flow, as in Batesian mimicry [2Pasteur G. A Classification Review of Mimicry Systems.Annu. Rev. Ecol. System. 1982; 13: 169-199Crossref Scopus (148) Google Scholar], and often reflecting antagonistic relationships such as predation. So what do animals tell us? The legend of Saint Francis of Assisi, who preached successfully to birds and wolves, and the children's book character of Dr Doolittle, remind us of the human quest to communicate across species boundaries and ‘understand’ animals sensu stricto. Ethologists in the 19th and 20th centuries made much progress listening carefully to a range of animals, often carrying sophisticated recording gear into their gardens and forests, deep into the jungle, and into the depth of oceans — so we have some idea, and in some cases a good idea, how birds talk to each other, and what about, what kind of messages monkeys exchange with their alarm calls, and why whales produce, to us at least, beautifully relaxing songs [3Catchpole C.K. Dittami J. Leisler B. Differential responses to male song repertoires in female songbirds implanted with oestradiol.Nature. 1984; 312: 563-564Crossref Scopus (125) Google Scholar, 4Ghazanfar A.A. Hauser M.D. The neuroethology of primate vocal communication: substrates for the evolution of speech.Trends Cogn. Sci. 1999; 3: 377-384Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (68) Google Scholar, 5Marino L. Connor R.C. Fordyce R.E. Herman L.M. Hof P.R. Lefebvre L. Lusseau D. McCowan B. Nimchinsky E.A. Pack A.A. et al.Cetaceans have complex brains for complex cognition.PLoS Biol. 2007; 5: e139Crossref PubMed Scopus (181) Google Scholar]. Much of this sophisticated spy-work was driven by the assumption so intuitive to the human mind that language is transmitted most effectively, most naturally and most easily through the acoustic channel. But there are other channels that enable rather different communication strategies. For instance, the private chemical (pheromone) channel that moths use to attract the opposite sex for mating [6David C.T. Kennedy J.S. Ludlow A.R. Finding of a sex pheromone source by gypsy moths released in the field.Nature. 1983; 303: 804-806Crossref Scopus (129) Google Scholar] requires very specific sensory filters on the part of the receiver. Visual signals usually do not rely on specific adaptations of the receiver's visual system, so this channel lends itself to some rather public statements to a wide audience — if such communication is to retain any secrets, an animal has to develop a specific signal lexicon that is highly visible, but cannot be decoded without prior knowledge. Which brings the ethologists back into the field, with their recording equipment and patient observation skills! Decoding the language of the honeybee, the waggle dance used to indicate the location of food sources to their sisters (a visuo-tactile communication strategy), is one of the flagship achievements of classical ethology [7Riley J.R. Greggers U. Smith A.D. Reynolds D.R. Menzel R. The flight paths of honeybees recruited by the waggle dance.Nature. 2005; 435: 205-207Crossref PubMed Scopus (254) Google Scholar]. Less well known, but equally spectacular, is the fancy foot-work of fiddler crabs which signal, by waving their impressive claws (Figure 1A), their species identity, their strength, their size and other useful facts of self-esteem [8Salmon M. Hyatt G.W. Communication.in: Vernberg F.J. Vernberg W.B. Biology of Crustacea: Behaviour and Ecology. Academic Press, New York1983: 1-40Google Scholar]. It may be tempting to associate such behaviour with certain aspects of human body language (Figure 1B), such as various forms of hand-waving in the context of greeting, dancing or threatening; at least in a fixed cultural context, it is easy to decode such signals without any particular training. But decoding other information requires specific skills and experience: appreciating the intricacies of general body language may benefit from the special training of a psychologist; reading lips or sign language requires a decent amount of exposure to this language; and most of us would probably be hopelessly lost at sea when trying to produce or read a semaphore distress call conveyed by colourful flag-waving. Whereas studies of visual communication strategies have often been confined to the standard sender–receiver model, the last few decades have seen a growing interest in the environmental factors that constrain signal detection [9Endler J.A. Signals, signal conditions, and the direction of evolution.Am. Nat. 1992; 139: S125-S153Crossref Scopus (1280) Google Scholar, 10Fleishman L.J. The influence of the sensory system and the environment on motion patterns in the visual displays of anoline lizards and other vertebrates.Am. Nat. 1992; 139: 36-61Crossref Scopus (204) Google Scholar]. Despite a general recognition that sensory processing — which defines the bottleneck of information transfer for the receiver — is critically linked to its ecological context, transmission channel properties are still little understood. A paper published recently in Current Biology[11Peters R.A. Hemmi J.M. Zeil J. Signalling against the wind: modifying motion signal structure in response to increased noise.Curr. Biol. 2007; 17: 1231-1234Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (73) Google Scholar] thus makes a notable contribution, focusing as it does on the relationship between motion vision and movement-based signaling. Peters et al.[11Peters R.A. Hemmi J.M. Zeil J. Signalling against the wind: modifying motion signal structure in response to increased noise.Curr. Biol. 2007; 17: 1231-1234Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (73) Google Scholar] studied the signal content of the visual communication gestures of lizards in their natural habitat. The tail-flicking displays of Jacky lizards in territorial disputes were recorded in their natural habitat, the Australian bush. The movement signal arising from the tail flick needs to be picked up by the motion processing system of the observer, and to be separated from motion signals generated by wind-blown vegetation behind the displaying animal. Earlier studies [12Zeil J. Zanker J.M. A glimpse into crabworld.Vision Res. 1997; 37: 3417-3426Crossref PubMed Scopus (46) Google Scholar, 13Peters R.A. Clifford C.W.G. Evans C. Measuring the structure of dynamic visual signals.Anim. Behav. 2002; 64: 131-146Crossref Scopus (60) Google Scholar] investigated the spatial and temporal structure of movement-based communication signals as seen against background motion noise with the aid of computational models of motion processing. Peters et al. [11Peters R.A. Hemmi J.M. Zeil J. Signalling against the wind: modifying motion signal structure in response to increased noise.Curr. Biol. 2007; 17: 1231-1234Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (73) Google Scholar] have made a big leap by experimentally manipulating wind speed, and consequently the background noise level that constrains the detection of the lizard tail-flick by a conspecific observer. Their surprising finding is that this particular lizard does not increase the speed of the flick in the presence of wind, as other lizards do [10Fleishman L.J. The influence of the sensory system and the environment on motion patterns in the visual displays of anoline lizards and other vertebrates.Am. Nat. 1992; 139: 36-61Crossref Scopus (204) Google Scholar], but rather it changes the temporal structure and the duration of the display. A number of exciting research questions arise from these observations: which aspects of environmental motion make the lizards adjust their choreography? What is the specific relationship between tail-flick dynamics and noise-signal distribution that leads to this particular signalling strategy? Which properties of low-level motion detectors and higher level motion integration mechanisms are required to optimise such signal detection in the presence of noise? Whatever the details that future work will uncover, the current paper demonstrates for the first time how a visual communication system is smartly adjusted to specific dynamic environmental conditions. The study of animal communication, which bewilders the scientist with its variety and complexity, has the opportunity to achieve a new level of understanding by considering the communication signal content in the context of the neural processing necessary to enable ‘secure’ communication in real life, which is dynamic, noisy and short. This task requires the classically trained ethologist to communicate and collaborate with researchers in diverse other fields, such as ecologists, physicists, sensory physiologists and computational modelers. So what has this genuinely cross-disciplinary approach in stall for us as scientific community, for our sponsors, for our society? Studying motion processing mechanisms under natural operating conditions can provide essential clues to understanding how complex distributions of local motion signals can be segmented into meaningful patterns [14Zanker J.M. Combining local motion signals: A computational study of segmentation and transparency.in: Zanker J.M. Zeil J. Motion Vision: Computational, Neural and Ecological Constraints. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg, New York2001: 113-124Crossref Google Scholar]. A deeper understanding of communication processes in other species will also provide new insights into the nature of human communication, its opportunities and limitations, and perhaps will even generate ideas for repairing or augmenting damaged or insufficient communication mechanisms. Comparative studies may be particularly helpful for analyzing body language in humans, a topic which has only recently seen the introduction of more rigorous quantitative methods, for instance, to investigate dynamic face perception [15O'Toole A.J. Roark D.A. Abdi H. Recognizing moving faces: a psychological and neural synthesis.Trends Cogn. Sci. 2002; 6: 261-266Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (346) Google Scholar]. Understanding how particular communication channels are optimized, how signal processing and signal production are shaped by external constraints, can further help to design sophisticated methods of signal extraction in a wide range of technical applications. And perhaps — blending the legend of Saint Francis, the fiction of Doolittle, and the passion of pet lovers into reality — we might eventually even be able to tap into animal communication channels and speak to the birds and the wolves." @default.
- W2081490497 created "2016-06-24" @default.
- W2081490497 creator A5039521852 @default.
- W2081490497 date "2007-09-01" @default.
- W2081490497 modified "2023-10-14" @default.
- W2081490497 title "Animal Communication: Reading Lizards' Body Language in Context" @default.
- W2081490497 cites W116903684 @default.
- W2081490497 cites W1984272147 @default.
- W2081490497 cites W1986310860 @default.
- W2081490497 cites W1995257155 @default.
- W2081490497 cites W2000942823 @default.
- W2081490497 cites W2024180715 @default.
- W2081490497 cites W2027403759 @default.
- W2081490497 cites W2049579850 @default.
- W2081490497 cites W2057794025 @default.
- W2081490497 cites W2061388279 @default.
- W2081490497 cites W2109786901 @default.
- W2081490497 cites W2116390372 @default.
- W2081490497 cites W2129942835 @default.
- W2081490497 cites W2151519196 @default.
- W2081490497 cites W597866242 @default.
- W2081490497 doi "https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2007.07.006" @default.
- W2081490497 hasPubMedId "https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/17878050" @default.
- W2081490497 hasPublicationYear "2007" @default.
- W2081490497 type Work @default.
- W2081490497 sameAs 2081490497 @default.
- W2081490497 citedByCount "1" @default.
- W2081490497 countsByYear W20814904972016 @default.
- W2081490497 crossrefType "journal-article" @default.
- W2081490497 hasAuthorship W2081490497A5039521852 @default.
- W2081490497 hasBestOaLocation W20814904971 @default.
- W2081490497 hasConcept C138885662 @default.
- W2081490497 hasConcept C151730666 @default.
- W2081490497 hasConcept C18903297 @default.
- W2081490497 hasConcept C2779343474 @default.
- W2081490497 hasConcept C41895202 @default.
- W2081490497 hasConcept C554936623 @default.
- W2081490497 hasConcept C78458016 @default.
- W2081490497 hasConcept C86803240 @default.
- W2081490497 hasConcept C90856448 @default.
- W2081490497 hasConceptScore W2081490497C138885662 @default.
- W2081490497 hasConceptScore W2081490497C151730666 @default.
- W2081490497 hasConceptScore W2081490497C18903297 @default.
- W2081490497 hasConceptScore W2081490497C2779343474 @default.
- W2081490497 hasConceptScore W2081490497C41895202 @default.
- W2081490497 hasConceptScore W2081490497C554936623 @default.
- W2081490497 hasConceptScore W2081490497C78458016 @default.
- W2081490497 hasConceptScore W2081490497C86803240 @default.
- W2081490497 hasConceptScore W2081490497C90856448 @default.
- W2081490497 hasIssue "18" @default.
- W2081490497 hasLocation W20814904971 @default.
- W2081490497 hasLocation W20814904972 @default.
- W2081490497 hasOpenAccess W2081490497 @default.
- W2081490497 hasPrimaryLocation W20814904971 @default.
- W2081490497 hasRelatedWork W2011812956 @default.
- W2081490497 hasRelatedWork W2016368704 @default.
- W2081490497 hasRelatedWork W2032521526 @default.
- W2081490497 hasRelatedWork W2033283896 @default.
- W2081490497 hasRelatedWork W2064901328 @default.
- W2081490497 hasRelatedWork W2090767971 @default.
- W2081490497 hasRelatedWork W2114544095 @default.
- W2081490497 hasRelatedWork W2350662507 @default.
- W2081490497 hasRelatedWork W2364704900 @default.
- W2081490497 hasRelatedWork W2928062689 @default.
- W2081490497 hasVolume "17" @default.
- W2081490497 isParatext "false" @default.
- W2081490497 isRetracted "false" @default.
- W2081490497 magId "2081490497" @default.
- W2081490497 workType "article" @default.