Matches in SemOpenAlex for { <https://semopenalex.org/work/W2106694006> ?p ?o ?g. }
Showing items 1 to 80 of
80
with 100 items per page.
- W2106694006 endingPage "195" @default.
- W2106694006 startingPage "177" @default.
- W2106694006 abstract "Abstract We present a study that links optimal foraging theory (OFT) to behavioral timing. OFT's distinguishing feature is the use of models that compute the most advantageous behavior for a particular foraging problem and compare the optimal solution to empirical data with little reference to psychological processes. The study of behavioral timing, in contrast, emphasizes performance in relation to time, most often without strategic or functional considerations. In three experiments, reinforcer-maximizing behavior and timing performance are identified and related to each other. In all three experiments starlings work in a setting that simulates food patches separated by a flying distance between the two perches. The patches contain a variable and unpredictable number of reinforcers and deplete suddenly without signal. Before depletion, patches deliver food at fixed intervals (FI). Our main dependent variables are the times of occurrence of three behaviors: the “peak” in pecking rate (Peak), the time of the last peck before “giving in” (GIT), and the time for “moving on” to a new patch (MOT). We manipulate travel requirement (Experiment 1), level of deprivation and FI (Experiment 2), and size of reinforcers (Experiment 3). For OFT, Peak should equal the FI in all conditions while GIT and MOT should just exceed it. Behavioral timing and Scalar Expectancy Theory (SET) in particular predict a Peak at around the FI and a longer (unspecified) GIT, and make no prediction for MOT. We found that Peak was close to the FI and GIT was approximately 1.5 times longer, neither being affected by travel, hunger, or reinforcer size manipulations. MOT varied between 1.5 and just over 3 times the FI, was responsive to both travel time and the FI, and did not change when the reinforcer rate was manipulated. These results support the practice of producing models that explicitly separate information available to the subject from strategic use of this information." @default.
- W2106694006 created "2016-06-24" @default.
- W2106694006 creator A5044293755 @default.
- W2106694006 creator A5054254590 @default.
- W2106694006 date "2002-02-01" @default.
- W2106694006 modified "2023-09-23" @default.
- W2106694006 title "Timing and Foraging: Gibbon's Scalar Expectancy Theory and Optimal Patch Exploitation" @default.
- W2106694006 cites W1974366296 @default.
- W2106694006 cites W1981093698 @default.
- W2106694006 cites W1984132513 @default.
- W2106694006 cites W1986409137 @default.
- W2106694006 cites W1998503232 @default.
- W2106694006 cites W2002984461 @default.
- W2106694006 cites W2029068666 @default.
- W2106694006 cites W2029996772 @default.
- W2106694006 cites W2032130729 @default.
- W2106694006 cites W2054183375 @default.
- W2106694006 cites W2060694206 @default.
- W2106694006 cites W2071837129 @default.
- W2106694006 cites W2072996399 @default.
- W2106694006 cites W2081512069 @default.
- W2106694006 cites W2093177513 @default.
- W2106694006 cites W2093702500 @default.
- W2106694006 cites W2142180526 @default.
- W2106694006 cites W2170411204 @default.
- W2106694006 cites W2319829320 @default.
- W2106694006 doi "https://doi.org/10.1006/lmot.2001.1110" @default.
- W2106694006 hasPublicationYear "2002" @default.
- W2106694006 type Work @default.
- W2106694006 sameAs 2106694006 @default.
- W2106694006 citedByCount "60" @default.
- W2106694006 countsByYear W21066940062012 @default.
- W2106694006 countsByYear W21066940062013 @default.
- W2106694006 countsByYear W21066940062014 @default.
- W2106694006 countsByYear W21066940062015 @default.
- W2106694006 countsByYear W21066940062016 @default.
- W2106694006 countsByYear W21066940062017 @default.
- W2106694006 countsByYear W21066940062018 @default.
- W2106694006 countsByYear W21066940062019 @default.
- W2106694006 countsByYear W21066940062020 @default.
- W2106694006 countsByYear W21066940062021 @default.
- W2106694006 countsByYear W21066940062022 @default.
- W2106694006 crossrefType "journal-article" @default.
- W2106694006 hasAuthorship W2106694006A5044293755 @default.
- W2106694006 hasAuthorship W2106694006A5054254590 @default.
- W2106694006 hasConcept C15744967 @default.
- W2106694006 hasConcept C165287380 @default.
- W2106694006 hasConcept C188353592 @default.
- W2106694006 hasConcept C18903297 @default.
- W2106694006 hasConcept C77805123 @default.
- W2106694006 hasConcept C86803240 @default.
- W2106694006 hasConcept C95812059 @default.
- W2106694006 hasConceptScore W2106694006C15744967 @default.
- W2106694006 hasConceptScore W2106694006C165287380 @default.
- W2106694006 hasConceptScore W2106694006C188353592 @default.
- W2106694006 hasConceptScore W2106694006C18903297 @default.
- W2106694006 hasConceptScore W2106694006C77805123 @default.
- W2106694006 hasConceptScore W2106694006C86803240 @default.
- W2106694006 hasConceptScore W2106694006C95812059 @default.
- W2106694006 hasIssue "1" @default.
- W2106694006 hasLocation W21066940061 @default.
- W2106694006 hasOpenAccess W2106694006 @default.
- W2106694006 hasPrimaryLocation W21066940061 @default.
- W2106694006 hasRelatedWork W2013032970 @default.
- W2106694006 hasRelatedWork W2014807630 @default.
- W2106694006 hasRelatedWork W2016607578 @default.
- W2106694006 hasRelatedWork W2018788837 @default.
- W2106694006 hasRelatedWork W2028476291 @default.
- W2106694006 hasRelatedWork W2035655022 @default.
- W2106694006 hasRelatedWork W2063591067 @default.
- W2106694006 hasRelatedWork W2067108088 @default.
- W2106694006 hasRelatedWork W2115643887 @default.
- W2106694006 hasRelatedWork W4246049162 @default.
- W2106694006 hasVolume "33" @default.
- W2106694006 isParatext "false" @default.
- W2106694006 isRetracted "false" @default.
- W2106694006 magId "2106694006" @default.
- W2106694006 workType "article" @default.