Matches in SemOpenAlex for { <https://semopenalex.org/work/W2108165294> ?p ?o ?g. }
Showing items 1 to 64 of
64
with 100 items per page.
- W2108165294 startingPage "38" @default.
- W2108165294 abstract "Temporal Spacing and Learning Hal Pashler, Doug Rohrer, and Nicholas J. Cepeda March 2006 Volume 19, 30, 38 Studies going back a century and more have found that spacing learning episodes across time sometimes enhances memory. The so- called spacing effect is the topic of hundreds of articles, and one might assume that we know all we need to know about it. However, the subtitle of an article on spacing effects that Frank Dempster published in American Psychologist in 1988 — A case study in the failure to apply the results of psychological research — remains appropriate now. Whether one looks at classrooms, instructional design texts, or language learning software, there is little sign that people are paying attention to temporal spacing of learning. Before pointing fingers, it is reasonable to ask: exactly what advice can we offer with confidence? Reviewing the literature, our research team (which includes John Wixted and Shana Carpenter from University of California, San Diego, along with the three of us), supported by Institute of Education Sciences concluded: not very much (Cepeda, et al., in press). Despite numerous papers, very few researchers have examined memory after retention intervals of even one week. In some pioneering studies, Harry Bahrick showed that long inter-study spacing can enhance learning over years, but he trained subjects to a criterion of mastery on each session, thus allowing study time to grow with spacing. All in all, we concluded that psychology could not yet offer specific advice about how to make the most efficient use of study time. Moreover, research has focused heavily on recall of word lists, and some have suggested that more complex or less rote forms of learning might not show spacing benefits at all. Our team has set about seeking to close some of these various gaps in knowledge to allow a translation of basic spacing research into practical contexts. Optimal Spacing Intervals In one recent study, we gave 161 subjects two learning sessions (separated by an inter-study interval, or ISI, from minutes to 3 months). Each session involved learning a set of obscure facts. Six months after the second session, subjects were brought back for a final test. Performance was best when the ISI was 10 to 20 percent of the retention interval (Cepeda et al., 2006). Similar results were found when the same subjects learned names of unusual objects depicted in photographs. In a study currently under way using the Web, more than 2,000 subjects are being trained at inter-study intervals from minutes out to one year, with a final test taking place after an additional year. While data are still accruing, the results seem to provide clear support for the approximate ratio rule described above. Furthermore, the benefits of spacing seem to grow ever larger as retention intervals are lengthened; thus, for one-year retention, a one-month spacing produces a three-fold or greater increase in memory as compared to a day or even a week of spacing. While increasing spacing too much always produces some decline, as earlier short- duration studies had implied, the decline is invariably quite modest. Therefore, to facilitate retention over years, it seems critical to space training over several months at least, but avoiding overly long spacing seems like a relatively minor concern. Mathematics Learning To move beyond these somewhat rote learning tasks, the University of South Florida wing of our team under the direction of Rohrer has been teaching students abstract mathematics skills (e.g., Rohrer & Taylor, 2006). In a study run with Kelli Taylor, students learned to solve a type of permutation problem, and then worked two sets of practice problems. One-week spacing separating the practice sets drastically improved final test performance (which involved problems not previously encountered). In fact, when the two practice sets were back-to-back, final performance was scarcely better than if the second study session was deleted altogether. This fits with other research from our team showing that benefits of over-learning decline sharply with time (Rohrer, et al., 2005). Interestingly, most mathematics textbooks follow precisely the approach that our studies find so ineffective: a brief lesson on a topic is followed by a practice set containing virtually every problem in the book relating to this topic. Far more useful, we suspect, is to intersperse problems related to older topics covered over past weeks and months. We have also been looking at spacing effects in acquisition of visuospatial skills, such as artificial categorization tasks and classification of skin lesions as arising from melanoma versus other conditions. Interestingly, we have thus far found little spacing benefit in this domain. Obviously, we have much more to learn about spacing, but from our results so far we are confident that the potential of spaced practice to reduce forgetting is enormous — and we are optimistic that an adequate empirical basis for choosing optimal timing of study sessions for practical purposes may finally be starting to emerge. References Cepeda, N. J., Mozer, M. C., Coburn, N., Rohrer, D., Wixted, J. T., & Pashler, H. (2006). Optimizing distributed practice: Theoretical analysis and practical implications. Manuscript submitted for publication. Cepeda, N. J., Pashler, H., Vul, E., Wixted, J. T., & Rohrer, D. (in press). Distributed practice in verbal recall tasks: A review and quantitative synthesis. Psychological Bulletin. Rohrer, D., & Taylor, K. (2006). The effects of overlearning and distributed practice on the retention of mathematics knowledge. Manuscript submitted for publication. Rohrer, D., Taylor, K., Pashler, H., Cepeda, N. J., & Wixted, J. T. (2005). The effect of overlearning on long-term retention. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19, 361-374. HAL PASHLER is a psychology professor at the University of California, San Diego. DOUG ROHRER is a professor of psychology at the University of South Florida. NICHOLAS J. CEPADA is a research associate at the University of Colorado at Boulder." @default.
- W2108165294 created "2016-06-24" @default.
- W2108165294 creator A5008952472 @default.
- W2108165294 creator A5038433616 @default.
- W2108165294 creator A5071554223 @default.
- W2108165294 date "2006-03-01" @default.
- W2108165294 modified "2023-09-26" @default.
- W2108165294 title "Temporal Spacing and Learning" @default.
- W2108165294 cites W2029665548 @default.
- W2108165294 cites W2049428464 @default.
- W2108165294 cites W2099013648 @default.
- W2108165294 hasPublicationYear "2006" @default.
- W2108165294 type Work @default.
- W2108165294 sameAs 2108165294 @default.
- W2108165294 citedByCount "1" @default.
- W2108165294 crossrefType "journal-article" @default.
- W2108165294 hasAuthorship W2108165294A5008952472 @default.
- W2108165294 hasAuthorship W2108165294A5038433616 @default.
- W2108165294 hasAuthorship W2108165294A5071554223 @default.
- W2108165294 hasConcept C111919701 @default.
- W2108165294 hasConcept C134306372 @default.
- W2108165294 hasConcept C136764020 @default.
- W2108165294 hasConcept C139676723 @default.
- W2108165294 hasConcept C145420912 @default.
- W2108165294 hasConcept C15744967 @default.
- W2108165294 hasConcept C180747234 @default.
- W2108165294 hasConcept C2779182362 @default.
- W2108165294 hasConcept C2780364048 @default.
- W2108165294 hasConcept C28093023 @default.
- W2108165294 hasConcept C33923547 @default.
- W2108165294 hasConcept C41008148 @default.
- W2108165294 hasConcept C77805123 @default.
- W2108165294 hasConceptScore W2108165294C111919701 @default.
- W2108165294 hasConceptScore W2108165294C134306372 @default.
- W2108165294 hasConceptScore W2108165294C136764020 @default.
- W2108165294 hasConceptScore W2108165294C139676723 @default.
- W2108165294 hasConceptScore W2108165294C145420912 @default.
- W2108165294 hasConceptScore W2108165294C15744967 @default.
- W2108165294 hasConceptScore W2108165294C180747234 @default.
- W2108165294 hasConceptScore W2108165294C2779182362 @default.
- W2108165294 hasConceptScore W2108165294C2780364048 @default.
- W2108165294 hasConceptScore W2108165294C28093023 @default.
- W2108165294 hasConceptScore W2108165294C33923547 @default.
- W2108165294 hasConceptScore W2108165294C41008148 @default.
- W2108165294 hasConceptScore W2108165294C77805123 @default.
- W2108165294 hasIssue "3" @default.
- W2108165294 hasLocation W21081652941 @default.
- W2108165294 hasOpenAccess W2108165294 @default.
- W2108165294 hasPrimaryLocation W21081652941 @default.
- W2108165294 hasRelatedWork W196989192 @default.
- W2108165294 hasRelatedWork W1972017335 @default.
- W2108165294 hasRelatedWork W2008908699 @default.
- W2108165294 hasRelatedWork W2033152059 @default.
- W2108165294 hasRelatedWork W2052154769 @default.
- W2108165294 hasRelatedWork W2154666168 @default.
- W2108165294 hasRelatedWork W2173682301 @default.
- W2108165294 hasRelatedWork W2335076397 @default.
- W2108165294 hasRelatedWork W3160777247 @default.
- W2108165294 hasVolume "19" @default.
- W2108165294 isParatext "false" @default.
- W2108165294 isRetracted "false" @default.
- W2108165294 magId "2108165294" @default.
- W2108165294 workType "article" @default.