Matches in SemOpenAlex for { <https://semopenalex.org/work/W2147745947> ?p ?o ?g. }
Showing items 1 to 87 of
87
with 100 items per page.
- W2147745947 startingPage "63" @default.
- W2147745947 abstract "... the Natives of this Country, of the antient original Race or Families, are distinguished by a Shibboleth upon their Tongues in pronouncing the Letter R ... (Defoe 1724-27: 232-233). ABSTRACT This article discusses the pronunciation of the phoneme/r/in early English. The traditional belief that the dominant pronunciation in Old and Middle English was [r] (an apical trill) is still supported by some authors, but there is growing consensus that there was a fairly wide range of /r/realisations already in early Germanic, and that the pronunciation of/r/ in Old English was about as variable as it is in present-day English. The article defends this view and goes a step further, suggesting that the modern distribution of variant pronunciations in British English reflects to some extent the distribution of very similar sounds in Old English. 1. Introduction: What makes a sound rhotic? The notion of rhotic is notoriously controversial and hard to formalise. A variety of segments with diverse articulations, including those symbolised as [[TEXT NOT REPRODUCIBLE IN ASCII.]], with or without diacritics, may be described as rhotic, though it is difficult to see what properties unify them as a natural class--if rhotics constitute such a class in the first place. To aggravate the taxonomic difficulties, there is a good deal of overlap (and of cross-linguistic ambiguity) between rhotics and other sounds. For example, the apico-alveolar tap [r], though usually regarded as a rhotic, may function as an allophone of apical stops (/t/ or /d/) in some languages (including American English), and sounds such as [[TEXT NOT REPRODUCIBLE IN ASCII.]] may well function as liquids in one language but as fricatives in another. (1) There have been attempts to define rhotics in terms of shared physical correlates, but the similarity between them, while hard to deny, is frustratingly elusive. For example, while many rhotics exhibit a conspicuously lowered third formant (F3) of their acoustic spectrum, such a dip is not characteric of rhotics in general: Uvular r-sounds have a high third formant, sometimes close to the fourth formant. Dental r-sounds also have a relatively high third formant, though not so high as the uvulars (Lindau 1985: 165). Some authors flatly deny the existence of any phonetic basis for defining rhotics; that is what Lindau emphasises in her seminal article: But there is no physical property that constitutes the essence of all rhotics (Lindau 1985: 166). One desperate solution, in the face of such difficulties, would be a cover feature with a circular definition (in brief, a is [+rhotic]), amounting in fact to an admission of defeat. According to Lindau (1985: 167), what underlies the class of rhotics is their phonological and family resemblance. Ladefoged--Maddieson (1996: 215) content themselves with a shockingly informal description of rhotics as a class of sounds unified mostly by historical connections and the choice of the letter r (or Greek [rho], or the phonemic symbol /r/) to represent them. Wiese (2001) proposes to define rhotics in terms of phonotactic patterning, as a point on the sonority scale between laterals and (semivowels), but, apart from the question whether such a definition is methodologically acceptable, it is far from evident that all are inherently more sonorous than all rhotics; many of the latter have glide realisations such as [[TEXT NOT REPRODUCIBLE IN ASCII.]] etc. Worse still, the universal sonority hierarchy may be at odds with the language-specific phonotactic behaviour of glides vs. liquids; in Old English, for example, /wr/ and /wl/ are permissible word-initial clusters. Oostendorp (2001) argues that rhotics have no fixed sonority value and are instead characterised by chameleonic adaptability: the more consonantal position a occupies, the less sonorous it is. …" @default.
- W2147745947 created "2016-06-24" @default.
- W2147745947 creator A5054691241 @default.
- W2147745947 date "2006-01-01" @default.
- W2147745947 modified "2023-10-18" @default.
- W2147745947 title "A Shibboleth upon Their Tongues: Early English /R/ Revisited" @default.
- W2147745947 cites W1572105959 @default.
- W2147745947 cites W157903397 @default.
- W2147745947 cites W1975344573 @default.
- W2147745947 cites W2003048847 @default.
- W2147745947 cites W2005526997 @default.
- W2147745947 cites W2014233880 @default.
- W2147745947 cites W2019912108 @default.
- W2147745947 cites W2032157126 @default.
- W2147745947 cites W2049871182 @default.
- W2147745947 cites W2053679433 @default.
- W2147745947 cites W2054097104 @default.
- W2147745947 cites W2059843986 @default.
- W2147745947 cites W2076323342 @default.
- W2147745947 cites W2165173456 @default.
- W2147745947 cites W2330989363 @default.
- W2147745947 cites W2496124103 @default.
- W2147745947 cites W2499122771 @default.
- W2147745947 cites W2500631892 @default.
- W2147745947 cites W3136945921 @default.
- W2147745947 cites W567683537 @default.
- W2147745947 cites W585026255 @default.
- W2147745947 cites W607457356 @default.
- W2147745947 cites W611381170 @default.
- W2147745947 cites W797318852 @default.
- W2147745947 cites W156947406 @default.
- W2147745947 cites W2483771666 @default.
- W2147745947 hasPublicationYear "2006" @default.
- W2147745947 type Work @default.
- W2147745947 sameAs 2147745947 @default.
- W2147745947 citedByCount "2" @default.
- W2147745947 countsByYear W21477459472018 @default.
- W2147745947 countsByYear W21477459472021 @default.
- W2147745947 crossrefType "journal-article" @default.
- W2147745947 hasAuthorship W2147745947A5054691241 @default.
- W2147745947 hasConcept C136197465 @default.
- W2147745947 hasConcept C138885662 @default.
- W2147745947 hasConcept C154945302 @default.
- W2147745947 hasConcept C2777212361 @default.
- W2147745947 hasConcept C2780522230 @default.
- W2147745947 hasConcept C2780844864 @default.
- W2147745947 hasConcept C41008148 @default.
- W2147745947 hasConcept C41895202 @default.
- W2147745947 hasConcept C95457728 @default.
- W2147745947 hasConceptScore W2147745947C136197465 @default.
- W2147745947 hasConceptScore W2147745947C138885662 @default.
- W2147745947 hasConceptScore W2147745947C154945302 @default.
- W2147745947 hasConceptScore W2147745947C2777212361 @default.
- W2147745947 hasConceptScore W2147745947C2780522230 @default.
- W2147745947 hasConceptScore W2147745947C2780844864 @default.
- W2147745947 hasConceptScore W2147745947C41008148 @default.
- W2147745947 hasConceptScore W2147745947C41895202 @default.
- W2147745947 hasConceptScore W2147745947C95457728 @default.
- W2147745947 hasLocation W21477459471 @default.
- W2147745947 hasOpenAccess W2147745947 @default.
- W2147745947 hasPrimaryLocation W21477459471 @default.
- W2147745947 hasRelatedWork W1480279980 @default.
- W2147745947 hasRelatedWork W1580230098 @default.
- W2147745947 hasRelatedWork W1849524010 @default.
- W2147745947 hasRelatedWork W1950007368 @default.
- W2147745947 hasRelatedWork W1969577828 @default.
- W2147745947 hasRelatedWork W1975344573 @default.
- W2147745947 hasRelatedWork W2070431486 @default.
- W2147745947 hasRelatedWork W2071314122 @default.
- W2147745947 hasRelatedWork W2071967801 @default.
- W2147745947 hasRelatedWork W2105293063 @default.
- W2147745947 hasRelatedWork W2114169843 @default.
- W2147745947 hasRelatedWork W2122802152 @default.
- W2147745947 hasRelatedWork W2138215484 @default.
- W2147745947 hasRelatedWork W2161900370 @default.
- W2147745947 hasRelatedWork W226648988 @default.
- W2147745947 hasRelatedWork W2335959328 @default.
- W2147745947 hasRelatedWork W2404824946 @default.
- W2147745947 hasRelatedWork W2922064407 @default.
- W2147745947 hasRelatedWork W3172794790 @default.
- W2147745947 hasRelatedWork W87108095 @default.
- W2147745947 hasVolume "42" @default.
- W2147745947 isParatext "false" @default.
- W2147745947 isRetracted "false" @default.
- W2147745947 magId "2147745947" @default.
- W2147745947 workType "article" @default.