Matches in SemOpenAlex for { <https://semopenalex.org/work/W2301881489> ?p ?o ?g. }
Showing items 1 to 77 of
77
with 100 items per page.
- W2301881489 abstract "Second Life (SL) is a virtual world application that enables users to create virtual representations of themselves and interact with other users. SL is increasingly being used to study important psychological questions. The current project sought to replicate within SL Asch’s (1951) classic finding of group influence, in which participants often respond in accordance with choices expressed by other members of a group, regardless of the accuracy of those choices. Participants were given a series of perceptual judgment trials, in which they chose one of three stimulus alternatives that matched the length of a target stimulus. Participants were tested either alone or with three other confederate avatars whose choices were predetermined by the experimenter. On two of the trials, confederate avatars unanimously chose incorrectly before the actual participant made their choice. Results showed that on these trials participants were significantly more likely to choose in accord with the confederate’s choices, relative to participants tested as single avatars. The results generally support earlier research on group influence and extend these findings to a virtual world environment. http://jvwresearch.org Group Influence on Choice 2 The Metaverse Assembled / Apr. 2013 Journal of Virtual Worlds Research Vol. 6, No. 1 1. Background and Introduction Second Life (http://secondlife.com) is a 3-D virtual environment, initially created as an experiment in virtual culture, society and worlds (Carpenter, 2009). SL users create virtual representations of themselves (i.e., avatars) and interact with other avatars in a shared virtual environment. A virtual world, although not completely without its own complications, offers an exciting and novel approach to both research and education. SL in particular offers users the opportunity to create entire environments limited only by the user’s imagination. A virtual world affords novel tools with which to create environments that do not or cannot exist in the real world, which in some cases relieves researchers of constraints and safety concerns often present in the physical world. Accordingly, many studies that are not feasible in real life can be conducted in a virtual world with minimal risk, costs, and complications; an advantage that becomes more prominent as our use of onlinetechnology increases. In order to realize its full potential, however, it is important to establish that a virtual world maintains important psychological similarities to real world situations. It has been suggested that immersing oneself in a virtual world with other individuals present, creates both a sense of ‘presence’ (e.g., the user’s avatar becomes an extension of themself immersed in the virtual environment) as well as a sense of ‘co-presence’ (e.g., the attribution of other avatars as extensions of other human agents and immersed in the same virtual shared space), creating a virtual experience that is in many ways comparable to the real world (Mennecke, Triplett, Hassall, Conde, & Heer, 2011). Although reasonable, the hypothesis that a virtual world gives users a sense of ‘presence’ as an avatar and ‘co-presence’ among other avatars, is ultimately an empirical question, and there have already been a number of studies in this area. One finding illustrating the value of a virtual world for social psychology is a study of virtual interpersonal distance (IPD). The results indicate that IPD phenomenon in the real world also appears in a virtual world, even with respect to differences among gender dyads and eye gaze (Bailenson, Beall, & Blascovich, 2002; Bailenson, Blascovich, Beall, & Loomis, 2001; Yee, Bailenson, Urbanek, Chang, & Merget, 2007). Other research has shown that the amount of IPD to an unknown virtual avatar is similar to the distance normally observed between two human strangers (Bailenson, Blascovich, & Guadagno, 2008). More directly relevant to the issue of ‘presence and co-presence’ are data suggesting that people perceive other avatars as extensions of other people. As an example, Hoyt, Blascovich, & Swinth (2003) conducted a study in which participants were told to perform a task in front of either human-controlled avatars, computer-controlled agents, or alone. Participants in the human-controlled group demonstrated the highest degree of social inhibition, thereby suggesting both the power of social influence on an individual and the extension of another human agent into a virtual avatar. Other research on self-perception has employed the use of what has been termed ‘immersive virtual environment technology’ (IVET), which allows researchers to manipulate the digital representation of an avatar’s appearance and control the avatar’s behavior using preset computer algorithms (Bailenson, Blascovich, Beall & Loomis, 2001; Loomis, Blascovich, & Beall, 1999). Being able to create an avatar that appears any way they wish, participants can also experience what it is like to perceive themselves as a different gender, ethnicity, or age, while interacting in a social situation. This kind of manipulation can prove a valuable assessment tool for a multitude of questions examined by psychologists, pertinent to social perception, empathy, aggression, personality, judgment, and other http://jvwresearch.org Group Influence on Choice 3 The Metaverse Assembled / Apr. 2013 Journal of Virtual Worlds Research Vol. 6, No. 1 social phenomena. Indeed, this area of research has already begun to make some striking contributions to social psychology (Bailenson, Blascovich, & Guadagno, 2008, for a review). Another strategy used to study social perception in a virtual world involves the passive use of avatars. In these studies the participant does not engage as an avatar themselves. Rather, they merely observe or interact with avatars from a real world perspective (i.e., observing avatars on a computer screen). One such study examined the classic obedience phenomenon introduced by Milgram (1963). In the original study, participants were presented with a situation in which they were told to administer shocks to another individual if that person answered incorrectly to a given problem. Participants were told to continue increasing the level of shock with each incorrect response, even when the individual expressed discomfort and pain, or ceased responding altogether. Of course, no real shocks were administered. Nonetheless, the reaction of participants ranged from discomfort to very high levels of anxiety, even though many of these same participants ended up administering the highest level of shock available. This type of study could not be replicated today because of ethical standards of research. The distress participants expressed when believing that their actions injured another human being is now ethically prohibited as an unacceptable risk. As an alternative strategy for examining what remains an interesting question -the degree to which humans obey the commands of another -a recent study replicated Milgram’s basic design and procedure within a virtual world. An avatar, rather than a real person, was the target for shocks administered by real-world participants. Interestingly, even though participants were aware that the individual they were shocking was not real, similar findings of increased anxiety and discomfort were recorded among participants. Of particular interest is that physiological measures (skin conductance and heart rate) indicated higher levels of distress when the avatar being shocked voiced their discomfort and pain than when they communicated solely via text chat (Slater, Antley, Davison, Swapp, Guger, Barker, Pistrang, & Sanchez-Vives , 2006). Studies of this type illustrate how a virtual world can offer many new research opportunities that are not possible, for various reasons, in real world situations. In order to better understand the possibilities and limitations of using SL and similar platforms to study psychological questions, more research is needed on the degree to which there is phenomenological symmetry between real world and virtual world situations. Our present study was intended to contribute to that objective. We attempted to replicate a basic finding from social psychology. In his classic study of social influence, Asch (1951) found that a significant number of participants would respond according to choices expressed by other members of their group, despite the fact that these answers were incorrect. A significant amount of pressure to conform occurs when the group is composed of three or more other people and others in the group are unanimous. Larsen, Triplett, Brant & Langenberg (1979) attributed the tendency of external members to conform as a learned behavior encouraged by social rewards. Our study sought to replicate Asch’s (1951) social influence study in a virtual world. Specifically, we examined whether SL participants would conform to the group’s choices when placed in a group situation and asked to perform alongside other avatars in perceptual judgment task, regardless of choice accuracy. http://jvwresearch.org Group Influence on Choice 4 The Metaverse Assembled / Apr. 2013 Journal of Virtual Worlds Research Vol. 6, No. 1 2. Method" @default.
- W2301881489 created "2016-06-24" @default.
- W2301881489 creator A5004207787 @default.
- W2301881489 creator A5020675085 @default.
- W2301881489 creator A5038161974 @default.
- W2301881489 creator A5041235330 @default.
- W2301881489 creator A5086944451 @default.
- W2301881489 date "2013-01-01" @default.
- W2301881489 modified "2023-09-24" @default.
- W2301881489 title "The Effect of Group Influence on Individual Choices in a Virtual Environment" @default.
- W2301881489 cites W1556348243 @default.
- W2301881489 cites W1879560794 @default.
- W2301881489 cites W1969900674 @default.
- W2301881489 cites W2005757068 @default.
- W2301881489 cites W2005774095 @default.
- W2301881489 cites W2067609776 @default.
- W2301881489 cites W2096108774 @default.
- W2301881489 cites W2116310671 @default.
- W2301881489 cites W2129162329 @default.
- W2301881489 cites W2133387828 @default.
- W2301881489 cites W2161569443 @default.
- W2301881489 cites W2581701216 @default.
- W2301881489 cites W3122490494 @default.
- W2301881489 cites W2591813341 @default.
- W2301881489 hasPublicationYear "2013" @default.
- W2301881489 type Work @default.
- W2301881489 sameAs 2301881489 @default.
- W2301881489 citedByCount "0" @default.
- W2301881489 crossrefType "journal-article" @default.
- W2301881489 hasAuthorship W2301881489A5004207787 @default.
- W2301881489 hasAuthorship W2301881489A5020675085 @default.
- W2301881489 hasAuthorship W2301881489A5038161974 @default.
- W2301881489 hasAuthorship W2301881489A5041235330 @default.
- W2301881489 hasAuthorship W2301881489A5086944451 @default.
- W2301881489 hasConcept C107457646 @default.
- W2301881489 hasConcept C15744967 @default.
- W2301881489 hasConcept C178790620 @default.
- W2301881489 hasConcept C185592680 @default.
- W2301881489 hasConcept C194969405 @default.
- W2301881489 hasConcept C2781311116 @default.
- W2301881489 hasConcept C41008148 @default.
- W2301881489 hasConcept C53332860 @default.
- W2301881489 hasConceptScore W2301881489C107457646 @default.
- W2301881489 hasConceptScore W2301881489C15744967 @default.
- W2301881489 hasConceptScore W2301881489C178790620 @default.
- W2301881489 hasConceptScore W2301881489C185592680 @default.
- W2301881489 hasConceptScore W2301881489C194969405 @default.
- W2301881489 hasConceptScore W2301881489C2781311116 @default.
- W2301881489 hasConceptScore W2301881489C41008148 @default.
- W2301881489 hasConceptScore W2301881489C53332860 @default.
- W2301881489 hasIssue "1" @default.
- W2301881489 hasLocation W23018814891 @default.
- W2301881489 hasOpenAccess W2301881489 @default.
- W2301881489 hasPrimaryLocation W23018814891 @default.
- W2301881489 hasRelatedWork W2102127289 @default.
- W2301881489 hasRelatedWork W2139883823 @default.
- W2301881489 hasRelatedWork W2226144950 @default.
- W2301881489 hasRelatedWork W2327148549 @default.
- W2301881489 hasRelatedWork W2502822721 @default.
- W2301881489 hasRelatedWork W2774157693 @default.
- W2301881489 hasRelatedWork W2789839258 @default.
- W2301881489 hasRelatedWork W2950022282 @default.
- W2301881489 hasRelatedWork W2955953746 @default.
- W2301881489 hasRelatedWork W2998647102 @default.
- W2301881489 hasRelatedWork W3046909035 @default.
- W2301881489 hasRelatedWork W3125361786 @default.
- W2301881489 hasRelatedWork W3126862150 @default.
- W2301881489 hasRelatedWork W3137624160 @default.
- W2301881489 hasRelatedWork W3138040260 @default.
- W2301881489 hasRelatedWork W59878244 @default.
- W2301881489 hasRelatedWork W2184079494 @default.
- W2301881489 hasRelatedWork W2479859220 @default.
- W2301881489 hasVolume "6" @default.
- W2301881489 isParatext "false" @default.
- W2301881489 isRetracted "false" @default.
- W2301881489 magId "2301881489" @default.
- W2301881489 workType "article" @default.