Matches in SemOpenAlex for { <https://semopenalex.org/work/W2326283386> ?p ?o ?g. }
- W2326283386 endingPage "333" @default.
- W2326283386 startingPage "325" @default.
- W2326283386 abstract "Psychological research has a long track record of developing tools for our understanding of mental processes. Prominent among these tools are the measurement of the duration of cognitive processes (mental chronometry) and the localization of these processes in the brain (cognitive neuroscience). Somewhat less prominent, but undeservedly so, is eye movement recording or eye-tracking. Eyetracking has a century-old history (Wade & Tatler, 2005) that has recently culminated in the widespread availability of relatively affordable and low-effort tools for the unobtrusive study of visual exploratory behaviour (e.g., Holmqvist et al., 2011). Eye movements, an endless succession of rapid jumps (saccades) and brief resting periods (fixations) of the eye balls, arguably constitute our most frequent goal-directed behaviour (cf. Desmurget, Pelisson, Rossetti, & Prablanc, 1998; Hayhoe & Ballard, 2005). Measurements of eye position can be taken several hundred times per second without requiring additional task instructions or interrupting the naturally occurring behaviour of participants. Successive measures with similar spatial coordinates are aggregated into one fixation with a specific location and duration; larger changes in location yield new fixations, and thus information about saccade directions and saccade sizes. Multiple fixations falling on a given visual object or area of interest can be further aggregated, resulting in gaze durations or total viewing times or dwell times as additional eye movement measures (Holmqvist et al., 2011). Importantly, just two simple assumptions about eye movement measures have allowed cognitive scientists to employ eye-tracking very productively in the service of cognitive research. First, gazing at something likely indicates the object of our thoughts (Tanenhaus, Spivey-Knowlton, Eberhard, & Sedivy, 1995). And secondly, the time spent looking at an object corresponds to the time we think about this object (Just & Carpenter, 1980). Both assumptions have received extensive support fromreading researchwheremostwords are successively fixated and fixation durations primarily reflect ease of comprehension (e.g., Kliegl, Nuthmann, & Engbert, 2006; Rayner, 1998; Rayner & Reingold, 2015; Reichle, Pollatsek, Fisher, & Rayner, 1998; Starr & Rayner, 2001). Building on these simple assumptions, eye-tracking thus provides detailed information about the spatial selectivity and temporal extent of ongoing cognition, enabling researchers to ‘‘read the mind’’. This research rationale can profitably be extended into the domain of numerical cognition. In this field of study the currently dominant view holds that number concepts are represented on a spatially oriented ‘‘mental number line’’, with small numbers represented to the left of larger numbers (at least in Western cultures; Dehaene, Bossini, & Giraux, 1993). The ‘‘mental number line’’ gives rise to spatial–numerical associations, which can be widely observed, both in simple number classification tasks and also in more complex tasks such as mental arithmetic (Fischer & Shaki, 2014). Eye movements provide online access to the focus of spatial attention and can therefore reveal the order and duration of concept activations, & Matthias Hartmann matthias.hartmann@psy.unibe.ch" @default.
- W2326283386 created "2016-06-24" @default.
- W2326283386 creator A5048822179 @default.
- W2326283386 creator A5075003968 @default.
- W2326283386 date "2016-02-29" @default.
- W2326283386 modified "2023-09-27" @default.
- W2326283386 title "Exploring the numerical mind by eye-tracking: a special issue" @default.
- W2326283386 cites W1124768102 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W1129118795 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W1485117386 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W1532079698 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W1650563059 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W1949881471 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W1969803959 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W1972326791 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W1974841121 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W1976339519 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W1976580685 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W1977334482 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W1979132393 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W1982084753 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W1984598749 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W1986405361 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W1989346493 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W1989444565 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W1996281302 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W1998972925 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2000392366 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2006510893 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2011216281 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2013112874 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2020755048 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2021499170 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2021682909 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2028629838 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2029570806 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2030330674 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2033629499 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2044608318 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2049389261 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2050540272 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2050860891 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2052885627 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2056172470 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2059731297 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2060053884 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2060144282 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2063146169 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2067253250 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2072372877 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2077140144 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2078279227 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2079146574 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2081558246 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2083019235 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2083408502 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2086766050 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2092274141 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2103050059 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2108566571 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2108858705 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2108950759 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2111291362 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2112849549 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2120985159 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2128667243 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2134781198 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2136582088 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2139551810 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2139968699 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2142107171 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2147299944 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2150329181 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2153076044 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2155154828 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2156806953 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2160077404 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2161391062 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2162304349 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2162324555 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2164101306 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2169670677 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2170328124 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2180905628 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2202054894 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2217379899 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2217876605 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2281233609 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2283884835 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2288768514 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2316782280 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W2533539899 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W25420487 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W340383892 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W4233550736 @default.
- W2326283386 cites W1465517599 @default.
- W2326283386 doi "https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0759-0" @default.
- W2326283386 hasPubMedId "https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/26927470" @default.