Matches in SemOpenAlex for { <https://semopenalex.org/work/W2399145959> ?p ?o ?g. }
Showing items 1 to 82 of
82
with 100 items per page.
- W2399145959 abstract "Preschoolers’ Trust is Sensitive to Variable Intentions Elena Hoicka (elena.hoicka@stir.ac.uk) Psychology Department, University of Sheffield, Western Bank, Sheffield, S10 2PT, UK Jessica Butcher, Felicity Malla Psychology, School of Natural Sciences, University of Stirling, Stirling, FK9 4LA, UK Paul L. Harris (paul_harris@gse.harvard.edu) Harvard Graduate School, Harvard University, Appian Way, Cambridge, MA, 02138, USA Abstract This research demonstrates that preschoolers flexibly trust and mistrust the same individuals, as preschoolers recognize that their intentions may vary. In Study 1 (N=101) 3- and 4-year- olds trusted speakers based on their current, rather than previous, intentions to give in/correct information. Thus preschoolers infer the meanings behind different intentions and recognize that intentions change within individuals over time. In Study 2 (N=80) 3- to 5-year-olds trusted speakers who were currently sincere, but previously intentionally inaccurate, rather than currently sincere, but previously ignorant, showing that preschoolers infer current knowledge from prior intentions. Preschoolers also trusted speakers who were currently knowledgeable, although previously ignorant, showing that they recognize knowledge is variable within individuals. Keywords: Trust, Intention, Knowledge, Frame Problem, Preschoolers, Humor Introduction A growing body of research suggests that children do not blindly trust just anyone; children consider whom to trust (e.g., Clement, Koenig & Harris, 2004; Corriveau, Meints & Harris, 2009; Koenig & Harris, 2005). However, in a world where people have variable knowledge and intentions, an important question is whether children are prepared to trust individuals on some occasions but not others (e.g., Nurmsoo & Robinson, 2009; Scofield & Behrend, 2008; Shafto, et al., 2012). Thus, the question becomes not just whom to trust but also when to trust a given person. This is important not only for how we acquire information, but also for how we dismiss uninformative or irrelevant information. This is the Frame Problem (e.g., Dennett, 1984), which is still proving difficult to solve in Artificial Intelligence (AI, e.g., Ekbia & Maguitman, 2001; Scherl & Levesque, 2003), but which may be a relatively easy problem for preschoolers to solve. Preschoolers consider past behaviors when deciding whom to trust. For example, when learning new words, both 3- and 4-year-olds trust a speaker who previously labeled familiar objects correctly over a speaker who labeled them incorrectly. Thus, children trust accurate over inaccurate speakers (e.g., Clement, et al., 2004; Koenig & Harris, 2005). Children also trust knowledgeable over ignorant actors (Einav & Robinson, 2011). Our first goal was to discover whether preschoolers trust an informant’s claims depending on their current intentions and not just on their previous accuracy or apparent knowledge. More specifically, we asked: (1) whether preschoolers are flexible when they trust others; (2) whether mental states, specifically intentions, play a role in trust; and (3) whether preschoolers understand that people’s intentions can change over time. Most of the research to date suggests that people who were previously accurate or knowledgeable can be trusted in future, whereas people who were previously inaccurate or ignorant cannot. However people are not statically trustworthy or untrustworthy (e.g., Nurmsoo & Robinson, 2009; Scofield & Behrend, 2008; Shafto et al., 2012). Rather, a person can be trustworthy at times, but not at others. Joking is a clear example of intentionally saying or doing the wrong thing (e.g., Hoicka, Jutsum, & Gattis, 2008; Leekam, 1991), and so it is an occasion when the audience should not trust the information provided. Indeed, jokers want their audience to know about their falsehood, and they do not expect the audience to believe any part of it (e.g., Leekam, 1991). Thus, people provide cues when they are joking (e.g., Hoicka & Gattis, 2012; Mireault, et al., 2012). In particular, parents express greater disbelief when joking as compared to acting literally (Hoicka, et al., 2008). Additionally, everyone jokes. For example, all 3-year-olds in a survey were reported by their parents to have produced novel jokes (Hoicka & Akhtar, 2012). Therefore, joking is an ideal way to examine whether preschoolers use intent to say or do the wrong thing as a cue not to learn. Research has started to consider whether young children understand the various contexts in which others intend to say or do the wrong thing. From 25 months, toddlers copy wrong actions marked as jokes (laughter), but correct the same wrong actions marked as mistakes (“Whoops!”; Hoicka & Gattis, 2008). From 30 months, toddlers copy jokers who mislabel familiar objects, but they do not copy people who sincerely mislabel (Hoicka & Akhtar, 2011). In the case of trust, children as young as 3 years understand that pretending is not a reliable cue for acquiring correct information compared to, for example, having direct experience with the relevant information (Koenig, 2012). A critical aspect of intention is that it is not a stable mental state. People’s intentions change over time (e.g.," @default.
- W2399145959 created "2016-06-24" @default.
- W2399145959 creator A5004633197 @default.
- W2399145959 creator A5013221510 @default.
- W2399145959 creator A5027677306 @default.
- W2399145959 creator A5039525707 @default.
- W2399145959 date "2013-01-01" @default.
- W2399145959 modified "2023-09-23" @default.
- W2399145959 title "Preschoolers’ Trust is Sensitive to Variable Intentions" @default.
- W2399145959 cites W1497793840 @default.
- W2399145959 cites W1513534564 @default.
- W2399145959 cites W1583904404 @default.
- W2399145959 cites W1872262600 @default.
- W2399145959 cites W2010820094 @default.
- W2399145959 cites W2034872352 @default.
- W2399145959 cites W2064874780 @default.
- W2399145959 cites W2070368455 @default.
- W2399145959 cites W2079359106 @default.
- W2399145959 cites W2084170892 @default.
- W2399145959 cites W2090436417 @default.
- W2399145959 cites W2092270167 @default.
- W2399145959 cites W2098472529 @default.
- W2399145959 cites W2111024652 @default.
- W2399145959 cites W2114159790 @default.
- W2399145959 cites W2122562655 @default.
- W2399145959 cites W2122905426 @default.
- W2399145959 cites W2132819119 @default.
- W2399145959 cites W2145606564 @default.
- W2399145959 cites W2149867945 @default.
- W2399145959 cites W2153544422 @default.
- W2399145959 cites W2155249922 @default.
- W2399145959 cites W2158598783 @default.
- W2399145959 cites W2431139695 @default.
- W2399145959 cites W2995559795 @default.
- W2399145959 hasPublicationYear "2013" @default.
- W2399145959 type Work @default.
- W2399145959 sameAs 2399145959 @default.
- W2399145959 citedByCount "0" @default.
- W2399145959 crossrefType "journal-article" @default.
- W2399145959 hasAuthorship W2399145959A5004633197 @default.
- W2399145959 hasAuthorship W2399145959A5013221510 @default.
- W2399145959 hasAuthorship W2399145959A5027677306 @default.
- W2399145959 hasAuthorship W2399145959A5039525707 @default.
- W2399145959 hasConcept C134306372 @default.
- W2399145959 hasConcept C15744967 @default.
- W2399145959 hasConcept C182365436 @default.
- W2399145959 hasConcept C33923547 @default.
- W2399145959 hasConcept C77805123 @default.
- W2399145959 hasConceptScore W2399145959C134306372 @default.
- W2399145959 hasConceptScore W2399145959C15744967 @default.
- W2399145959 hasConceptScore W2399145959C182365436 @default.
- W2399145959 hasConceptScore W2399145959C33923547 @default.
- W2399145959 hasConceptScore W2399145959C77805123 @default.
- W2399145959 hasIssue "35" @default.
- W2399145959 hasLocation W23991459591 @default.
- W2399145959 hasOpenAccess W2399145959 @default.
- W2399145959 hasPrimaryLocation W23991459591 @default.
- W2399145959 hasRelatedWork W1750393761 @default.
- W2399145959 hasRelatedWork W1965503673 @default.
- W2399145959 hasRelatedWork W1978485078 @default.
- W2399145959 hasRelatedWork W1979419554 @default.
- W2399145959 hasRelatedWork W2001421144 @default.
- W2399145959 hasRelatedWork W2017836055 @default.
- W2399145959 hasRelatedWork W2020702366 @default.
- W2399145959 hasRelatedWork W2034988253 @default.
- W2399145959 hasRelatedWork W2043764297 @default.
- W2399145959 hasRelatedWork W2045579638 @default.
- W2399145959 hasRelatedWork W2055642223 @default.
- W2399145959 hasRelatedWork W2063947317 @default.
- W2399145959 hasRelatedWork W2111314944 @default.
- W2399145959 hasRelatedWork W2118502649 @default.
- W2399145959 hasRelatedWork W2123858405 @default.
- W2399145959 hasRelatedWork W228336800 @default.
- W2399145959 hasRelatedWork W2618549736 @default.
- W2399145959 hasRelatedWork W3124841376 @default.
- W2399145959 hasRelatedWork W2051389941 @default.
- W2399145959 hasRelatedWork W37981365 @default.
- W2399145959 hasVolume "35" @default.
- W2399145959 isParatext "false" @default.
- W2399145959 isRetracted "false" @default.
- W2399145959 magId "2399145959" @default.
- W2399145959 workType "article" @default.