Matches in SemOpenAlex for { <https://semopenalex.org/work/W2467709311> ?p ?o ?g. }
Showing items 1 to 75 of
75
with 100 items per page.
- W2467709311 abstract "Discourse markers (DMs) are usually broadly defined as structuring and organizing devices that help produce and understand meaning-in-context (e.g. Schiffrin 1987, Fischer 2000). The panel of functions that they can perform ranges from local cohesion (e.g. a causal relation between (sub)clauses) to “global coherence” (Lenk 1998, e.g. topic shift), hence a great variability in the size and type of units they apply to. The notion of scope of DMs has received some attention in grammaticalization studies (Traugott 1995) and more recently in studies on position and segmentation (Pons Borderia & Estelles 2009, Estelles & Pons Borderia 2014). However, scope remains a complex feature to annotate directly in spoken corpora, given the interplay of syntactic and semantic-pragmatic interpretation it involves. I claim that a scale of DM scope can be obtained by merging the information provided by a multi-layered annotation of formal and functional features of DMs, here applied to a comparable corpus of spoken French and English. These annotations include (but are not limited to): function(s) of the DM; a three-fold positioning system (clause, dependency structure, turn-of-speech); co-occurrence patterns; disfluencies in the immediate co-text of the DM (silent/filled pauses, repetitions etc.). My proposal is that corpus-based annotations – coupled with metadata on the contextual settings (e.g. degree of preparation, number of speakers) – can be interpreted in terms of scopes and their relative cognitive load. I will focus on “sequential” DMs (Gonzalez 2005, Crible subm.), which form a functional subgroup attending to topic management and dialogue structure, viz. turn opening, turn closing, topic shifting, topic resuming, and listing (see Crible 2014 for operational definitions of these values), to examine the different ways in which they relate to co-text and context. According to Roberts & Krisner (2000), sequential DMs should theoretically be more complex to produce, given that they work on both the mental “linearization” (Levelt 1989) of the global order of segments (e.g. macro-structure of topics) and on the actual “linearity” of the articulated output (e.g. local transition between topics or turns). I will therefore look for local and contextual evidence for heavy cognitive load, viz. presence of disfluencies, number and function(s) of co-occurring DMs, formality and complexity of the situation (e.g. transmission of information, professional setting). My hypothesis is that sequential DMs should co-occur with more disfluencies (including other DMs) than DMs from other (ideational, interpersonal) groups. I also expect that those markers which are associated with heavy cognitive load take scope over larger units (several propositions, a whole turn). This corpus-based in-depth investigation of DMs in a variety of contexts should uncover patterns and clusters of features that provide indirect evidence for the scope of these structuring elements of discourse. This will in turn contribute to better define the notion of speech linearity, and bring us closer to a cognitive-pragmatic model of discourse structure, following the lines of Van Dijk (e.g. 1989), Degand & Simon (2009) or the Val.Es.Co group (e.g. Pons Borderia 2006). References Crible, L. (subm.). “Towards an operational category of discourse markers: A definition and its model.” Crible, L. 2014. “Identifying and describing discourse markers in spoken corpora. Annotation protocol v.8”. Technical report, Universite Catholique de Louvain. Degand, L. & Simon, A.-C. 2009. “Mapping prosody and syntax as discourse strategies: How Basic Discourse Units vary across genres”. In A. Wichmann, D. Barth-Weingarten, N. Dehe (eds), Where prosody meets pragmatics: research at the interface, Emerald: Bingley: 79-105. Estelles, M. & Pons Borderia, S. 2014. “Absolute initial position”. In S. Pons Borderia (ed.), Discourse Segmentation in Romance Languages, Amsterdam, John Benjamins: 121-155. Fischer, K. 2000. From cognitive semantics to lexical pragmatics. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Gonzalez, M. 2005. “Pragmatic markers and discourse coherence relations in English and Catalan oral narrative”. Discourse Studies 77/1: 53-86. Lenk, U. 1998. “Discourse markers and global coherence in conversation”. Journal of Pragmatics 30: 245-257. Levelt, W. 1989. Speaking. From intention to articulation. Cambridge: MIT Press. Pons Borderia, S. 2006. “A functional approach to the study of discourse markers”. In K. Fischer (ed.), Approaches to discourse particles, Amsterdam, Elsevier: 77-100. Pons Borderia, S. & Estelles, M. “Expressing digression linguistically: Do digressive markers exist?”. Journal of Pragmatics 41: 921-936. Roberts, B. & Kirsner, K. 2000. “Temporal cycles in speech production”. Language and cognitive processes” 15/2, 129-157. Schiffrin, D. 1987. Discourse markers. Cambridge: CUP. Traugott, E. 1995. “The role of the development of discourse markers in a theory of grammaticalization”. Paper presented at the International Conference on Historical Linguistics (ICHL) XII, Manchester, UK. Van Dijk, T. 1989. “Cognitive context models and discourse”. In M. Stamenow (ed.), Language structure, discourse and the access to consciousness, Amsterdam: John Benjamins: 189-226." @default.
- W2467709311 created "2016-07-22" @default.
- W2467709311 creator A5039098109 @default.
- W2467709311 date "2016-01-01" @default.
- W2467709311 modified "2023-09-27" @default.
- W2467709311 title "Position and context of structuring discourse markers. A corpus-based investigation of scope and cognitive load in French and English" @default.
- W2467709311 hasPublicationYear "2016" @default.
- W2467709311 type Work @default.
- W2467709311 sameAs 2467709311 @default.
- W2467709311 citedByCount "0" @default.
- W2467709311 crossrefType "journal-article" @default.
- W2467709311 hasAuthorship W2467709311A5039098109 @default.
- W2467709311 hasConcept C10138342 @default.
- W2467709311 hasConcept C104054115 @default.
- W2467709311 hasConcept C135756266 @default.
- W2467709311 hasConcept C138885662 @default.
- W2467709311 hasConcept C154945302 @default.
- W2467709311 hasConcept C15744967 @default.
- W2467709311 hasConcept C162324750 @default.
- W2467709311 hasConcept C166957645 @default.
- W2467709311 hasConcept C178790620 @default.
- W2467709311 hasConcept C185592680 @default.
- W2467709311 hasConcept C199360897 @default.
- W2467709311 hasConcept C204321447 @default.
- W2467709311 hasConcept C2775945657 @default.
- W2467709311 hasConcept C2778012447 @default.
- W2467709311 hasConcept C2779343474 @default.
- W2467709311 hasConcept C41008148 @default.
- W2467709311 hasConcept C41895202 @default.
- W2467709311 hasConcept C95457728 @default.
- W2467709311 hasConceptScore W2467709311C10138342 @default.
- W2467709311 hasConceptScore W2467709311C104054115 @default.
- W2467709311 hasConceptScore W2467709311C135756266 @default.
- W2467709311 hasConceptScore W2467709311C138885662 @default.
- W2467709311 hasConceptScore W2467709311C154945302 @default.
- W2467709311 hasConceptScore W2467709311C15744967 @default.
- W2467709311 hasConceptScore W2467709311C162324750 @default.
- W2467709311 hasConceptScore W2467709311C166957645 @default.
- W2467709311 hasConceptScore W2467709311C178790620 @default.
- W2467709311 hasConceptScore W2467709311C185592680 @default.
- W2467709311 hasConceptScore W2467709311C199360897 @default.
- W2467709311 hasConceptScore W2467709311C204321447 @default.
- W2467709311 hasConceptScore W2467709311C2775945657 @default.
- W2467709311 hasConceptScore W2467709311C2778012447 @default.
- W2467709311 hasConceptScore W2467709311C2779343474 @default.
- W2467709311 hasConceptScore W2467709311C41008148 @default.
- W2467709311 hasConceptScore W2467709311C41895202 @default.
- W2467709311 hasConceptScore W2467709311C95457728 @default.
- W2467709311 hasLocation W24677093111 @default.
- W2467709311 hasOpenAccess W2467709311 @default.
- W2467709311 hasPrimaryLocation W24677093111 @default.
- W2467709311 hasRelatedWork W110543391 @default.
- W2467709311 hasRelatedWork W196535143 @default.
- W2467709311 hasRelatedWork W2172083669 @default.
- W2467709311 hasRelatedWork W2300199732 @default.
- W2467709311 hasRelatedWork W2322372325 @default.
- W2467709311 hasRelatedWork W2520578292 @default.
- W2467709311 hasRelatedWork W2547019720 @default.
- W2467709311 hasRelatedWork W2761735392 @default.
- W2467709311 hasRelatedWork W2784001355 @default.
- W2467709311 hasRelatedWork W2889627301 @default.
- W2467709311 hasRelatedWork W2890263080 @default.
- W2467709311 hasRelatedWork W2894705513 @default.
- W2467709311 hasRelatedWork W2904573204 @default.
- W2467709311 hasRelatedWork W2945071864 @default.
- W2467709311 hasRelatedWork W3010557011 @default.
- W2467709311 hasRelatedWork W3040853565 @default.
- W2467709311 hasRelatedWork W3204252482 @default.
- W2467709311 hasRelatedWork W3212276285 @default.
- W2467709311 hasRelatedWork W846627010 @default.
- W2467709311 hasRelatedWork W2066542873 @default.
- W2467709311 isParatext "false" @default.
- W2467709311 isRetracted "false" @default.
- W2467709311 magId "2467709311" @default.
- W2467709311 workType "article" @default.