Matches in SemOpenAlex for { <https://semopenalex.org/work/W2513866468> ?p ?o ?g. }
- W2513866468 endingPage "759" @default.
- W2513866468 startingPage "748" @default.
- W2513866468 abstract "Children and adults have systematic preferences for some explanations over others. They favor explanations with ‘explanatory virtues’, such as simplicity and breadth, especially when direct probabilistic evidence is unavailable. Explanatory virtues are difficult to quantify and precisely define, but advances in philosophy and experimental psychology are reinvigorating the study of explanations and their role in cognition. Explanatory preferences are consequential because explanation plays a crucial role in inference and learning. We often infer to the best explanation, or learn by explaining to others or ourselves. When people engage in such explanation-based processes, explanatory virtues are recruited as evaluative constraints. As a result, explaining can facilitate the discovery of simple hypotheses and broad patterns, but it can also lead to systematic errors. Explanations play an important role in learning and inference. People often learn by seeking explanations, and they assess the viability of hypotheses by considering how well they explain the data. An emerging body of work reveals that both children and adults have strong and systematic intuitions about what constitutes a good explanation, and that these explanatory preferences have a systematic impact on explanation-based processes. In particular, people favor explanations that are simple and broad, with the consequence that engaging in explanation can shape learning and inference by leading people to seek patterns and favor hypotheses that support broad and simple explanations. Given the prevalence of explanation in everyday cognition, understanding explanation is therefore crucial to understanding learning and inference. Explanations play an important role in learning and inference. People often learn by seeking explanations, and they assess the viability of hypotheses by considering how well they explain the data. An emerging body of work reveals that both children and adults have strong and systematic intuitions about what constitutes a good explanation, and that these explanatory preferences have a systematic impact on explanation-based processes. In particular, people favor explanations that are simple and broad, with the consequence that engaging in explanation can shape learning and inference by leading people to seek patterns and favor hypotheses that support broad and simple explanations. Given the prevalence of explanation in everyday cognition, understanding explanation is therefore crucial to understanding learning and inference." @default.
- W2513866468 created "2016-09-16" @default.
- W2513866468 creator A5052150259 @default.
- W2513866468 date "2016-10-01" @default.
- W2513866468 modified "2023-10-17" @default.
- W2513866468 title "Explanatory Preferences Shape Learning and Inference" @default.
- W2513866468 cites W1177888723 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W1903964690 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W1913870337 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W1936290004 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W1968843430 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W1976079578 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W1984737313 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W1985257401 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W1986469870 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W1988271510 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W1991271408 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2003402500 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2016030371 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2017294267 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2020857233 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2026834355 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2034637247 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2036202604 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2039933150 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2041029272 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2044069304 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2061763978 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2068569099 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2073307787 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2075393375 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2081480835 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2084299944 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2092792044 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2099908396 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2107174604 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2108403623 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2110085097 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2112828004 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2117829646 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2117852736 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2118045142 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2119858020 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2119991646 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2125782830 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2126909304 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2131367314 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2140107468 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2144544542 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2148198315 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2154348687 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2155160391 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2171430159 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2257736704 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2263015629 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2275593974 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2298699437 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2322579942 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2346342990 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2469053887 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2479107154 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2538152482 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W2791630724 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W3165003809 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W4214546653 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W4239816899 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W4313429371 @default.
- W2513866468 cites W586288162 @default.
- W2513866468 doi "https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.08.001" @default.
- W2513866468 hasPubMedId "https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/27567318" @default.
- W2513866468 hasPublicationYear "2016" @default.
- W2513866468 type Work @default.
- W2513866468 sameAs 2513866468 @default.
- W2513866468 citedByCount "110" @default.
- W2513866468 countsByYear W25138664682016 @default.
- W2513866468 countsByYear W25138664682017 @default.
- W2513866468 countsByYear W25138664682018 @default.
- W2513866468 countsByYear W25138664682019 @default.
- W2513866468 countsByYear W25138664682020 @default.
- W2513866468 countsByYear W25138664682021 @default.
- W2513866468 countsByYear W25138664682022 @default.
- W2513866468 countsByYear W25138664682023 @default.
- W2513866468 crossrefType "journal-article" @default.
- W2513866468 hasAuthorship W2513866468A5052150259 @default.
- W2513866468 hasConcept C111472728 @default.
- W2513866468 hasConcept C138885662 @default.
- W2513866468 hasConcept C149782125 @default.
- W2513866468 hasConcept C15744967 @default.
- W2513866468 hasConcept C158600405 @default.
- W2513866468 hasConcept C162324750 @default.
- W2513866468 hasConcept C169760540 @default.
- W2513866468 hasConcept C169900460 @default.
- W2513866468 hasConcept C180747234 @default.
- W2513866468 hasConcept C188147891 @default.
- W2513866468 hasConcept C2776214188 @default.
- W2513866468 hasConcept C2776372474 @default.
- W2513866468 hasConcept C2777402642 @default.
- W2513866468 hasConcept C2778638050 @default.