Matches in SemOpenAlex for { <https://semopenalex.org/work/W2577753386> ?p ?o ?g. }
Showing items 1 to 98 of
98
with 100 items per page.
- W2577753386 abstract "Horizontal Trumps Vertical in the Spatial Organization of Numerical Magnitude Kevin J. Holmes (kevin.holmes@emory.edu) Stella F. Lourenco (stella.lourenco@emory.edu) Department of Psychology, Emory University 36 Eagle Row, Atlanta, GA 30322 Abstract The vast majority of research on the spatial nature of numerical representation has focused on the horizontal axis, highlighting the left-to-right orientation of increasing values (i.e., the mental number line). Recent evidence points to spatial organization along the vertical (bottom-to-top) axis as well. We argue, however, that these findings are better characterized as generalized magnitude mappings between number and other magnitude dimensions (e.g., near-far spatial extent), rather than genuine vertical orientation. Here we replicate generalized magnitude mappings for number (Exp. 1A) and show that they take precedence over left-to-right orientation (Exp. 1B), likely because of the direct mapping between dimensions. In contrast, we find no evidence of spontaneous organization along the true vertical axis (Exp. 2A), and left-to-right orientation trumped bottom-to-top orientation when the two were inconsistent with each other (Exp. 2B). Reliable bottom-to-top orientation was evident only after priming of magnitude relations among numbers (Exp. 3). Together, these findings demonstrate that number is more strongly represented horizontally than vertically. We suggest that experience with cultural tools may drive this asymmetry, and we highlight other cognitive and environmental factors that may influence the mental organization of magnitude dimensions beyond number. Keywords: number; spatial organization; SNARC effect; mental magnitude line. Introduction Across a wide range of common cultural tools and artifacts, numbers are spatially organized. Many of these external symbolic representations depict numbers horizontally (e.g., rulers, measuring tapes), but some do so vertically (e.g., mercury thermometers, measuring cups). Despite this variation in spatial forms, the vast majority of research on the mental association between number and space has focused almost exclusively on the horizontal axis. Many such studies have suggested that numerical representations take the form of a mental number line, running from left to right in representational space (at least in Western cultures; for reviews, see Fias & Fischer, 2005; Hubbard et al., 2005), without examining other spatial axes on which number might also be mentally organized. Recent findings suggest, however, that the vertical axis (specifically, bottom-to-top orientation) may also be recruited in the mental organization of number. In a parity judgment task, with response buttons arranged vertically on a tabletop, Ito and Hatta (2004) found that as numerical magnitude increased, “top” responses became faster relative to “bottom” responses (see also Gevers et al., 2006; Muller & Schwarz, 2007). Using saccadic latency as a dependent measure, Schwarz and Keus (2004) found that larger numbers elicited faster upward (relative to downward) saccades, and Loetscher et al. (2010) showed that vertical (not just horizontal) changes in participants’ eye position were predictive of the magnitude of numbers they subsequently generated. While such findings have been regarded as evidence for vertical orientation of number, they are also compatible with the notion that number is but one component of a more general system of magnitude representation (Walsh, 2003). On this account, different dimensions of magnitude are spontaneously aligned, producing generalized mappings of more/less relations across dimensions (e.g., number, duration, size). As such mappings have been observed even in preverbal infants (Lourenco & Longo, 2010), they do not appear to require the same degree of cultural support as organization of number along spatial axes (e.g., left-to-right orientation), and hence may be especially robust. In the case of previous findings for vertical orientation, generalized magnitude mappings can fully account for the observed effects without invoking spatial axes, or a specific orientation, at all. That is, numerical magnitude may have been mapped to another magnitude dimension rather than to the vertical axis per se. In studies using tabletop responses, number may have been mapped to distance, given that response buttons were not arranged along the true vertical bottom-top axis (i.e., sagittal plane). Instead, they differed in relative spatial extent from the body (i.e., transverse plane; see Fig. 1); the “top” and “bottom” responses in these studies may be better characterized as “far” and “near” responses, respectively. Thus, larger numerical values may have been associated with greater distance, rather than upward locations in space. Generalized magnitude mappings may also underlie the effects observed with saccadic measures. In these studies, number may be associated with effort, since upward and downward saccades may be considered more and less effortful, respectively. Vision research has shown that upward saccades are slower than downward saccades, and the resting position of the eyes is slightly lower than the actual vertical midpoint (Collewijn, Erkelens, & Steinman, 1988). Overall downward shifts in eye position have also been observed during number bisection tasks (Loetscher, Bockisch, & Brugger, 2008), suggesting that upward saccades may require greater effort, particularly during number processing. Thus, in studies using saccadic measures to examine vertical orientation, larger numerical values may have been associated with more effortful bodily actions (as has been shown for grasping; e.g., Lindemann et al., 2007). Given that the vertical axis has been confounded with other magnitude dimensions in previous studies, whether number is" @default.
- W2577753386 created "2017-01-26" @default.
- W2577753386 creator A5019554329 @default.
- W2577753386 creator A5023446441 @default.
- W2577753386 date "2011-01-01" @default.
- W2577753386 modified "2023-09-23" @default.
- W2577753386 title "Horizontal Trumps Vertical in the Spatial Organization of Numerical Magnitude" @default.
- W2577753386 cites W1520020354 @default.
- W2577753386 cites W1560635040 @default.
- W2577753386 cites W1966163033 @default.
- W2577753386 cites W1972422252 @default.
- W2577753386 cites W2001059750 @default.
- W2577753386 cites W2014144031 @default.
- W2577753386 cites W2019041952 @default.
- W2577753386 cites W2026945003 @default.
- W2577753386 cites W2052417512 @default.
- W2577753386 cites W2077140144 @default.
- W2577753386 cites W2082641819 @default.
- W2577753386 cites W2083019235 @default.
- W2577753386 cites W2129122129 @default.
- W2577753386 cites W2152633749 @default.
- W2577753386 cites W2162324555 @default.
- W2577753386 cites W2440054266 @default.
- W2577753386 cites W2512237710 @default.
- W2577753386 cites W569907279 @default.
- W2577753386 hasPublicationYear "2011" @default.
- W2577753386 type Work @default.
- W2577753386 sameAs 2577753386 @default.
- W2577753386 citedByCount "1" @default.
- W2577753386 countsByYear W25777533862013 @default.
- W2577753386 crossrefType "journal-article" @default.
- W2577753386 hasAuthorship W2577753386A5019554329 @default.
- W2577753386 hasAuthorship W2577753386A5023446441 @default.
- W2577753386 hasConcept C120665830 @default.
- W2577753386 hasConcept C121332964 @default.
- W2577753386 hasConcept C126691448 @default.
- W2577753386 hasConcept C1276947 @default.
- W2577753386 hasConcept C15744967 @default.
- W2577753386 hasConcept C16345878 @default.
- W2577753386 hasConcept C169760540 @default.
- W2577753386 hasConcept C169900460 @default.
- W2577753386 hasConcept C175779824 @default.
- W2577753386 hasConcept C18903297 @default.
- W2577753386 hasConcept C2524010 @default.
- W2577753386 hasConcept C2776502983 @default.
- W2577753386 hasConcept C33923547 @default.
- W2577753386 hasConcept C38976095 @default.
- W2577753386 hasConcept C45768438 @default.
- W2577753386 hasConcept C59218005 @default.
- W2577753386 hasConcept C62520636 @default.
- W2577753386 hasConcept C86803240 @default.
- W2577753386 hasConceptScore W2577753386C120665830 @default.
- W2577753386 hasConceptScore W2577753386C121332964 @default.
- W2577753386 hasConceptScore W2577753386C126691448 @default.
- W2577753386 hasConceptScore W2577753386C1276947 @default.
- W2577753386 hasConceptScore W2577753386C15744967 @default.
- W2577753386 hasConceptScore W2577753386C16345878 @default.
- W2577753386 hasConceptScore W2577753386C169760540 @default.
- W2577753386 hasConceptScore W2577753386C169900460 @default.
- W2577753386 hasConceptScore W2577753386C175779824 @default.
- W2577753386 hasConceptScore W2577753386C18903297 @default.
- W2577753386 hasConceptScore W2577753386C2524010 @default.
- W2577753386 hasConceptScore W2577753386C2776502983 @default.
- W2577753386 hasConceptScore W2577753386C33923547 @default.
- W2577753386 hasConceptScore W2577753386C38976095 @default.
- W2577753386 hasConceptScore W2577753386C45768438 @default.
- W2577753386 hasConceptScore W2577753386C59218005 @default.
- W2577753386 hasConceptScore W2577753386C62520636 @default.
- W2577753386 hasConceptScore W2577753386C86803240 @default.
- W2577753386 hasIssue "33" @default.
- W2577753386 hasLocation W25777533861 @default.
- W2577753386 hasOpenAccess W2577753386 @default.
- W2577753386 hasPrimaryLocation W25777533861 @default.
- W2577753386 hasRelatedWork W1968468117 @default.
- W2577753386 hasRelatedWork W1976962433 @default.
- W2577753386 hasRelatedWork W1982238157 @default.
- W2577753386 hasRelatedWork W1986287370 @default.
- W2577753386 hasRelatedWork W2007498565 @default.
- W2577753386 hasRelatedWork W2033714207 @default.
- W2577753386 hasRelatedWork W2037205197 @default.
- W2577753386 hasRelatedWork W2061346839 @default.
- W2577753386 hasRelatedWork W2075910156 @default.
- W2577753386 hasRelatedWork W2087398262 @default.
- W2577753386 hasRelatedWork W2091896418 @default.
- W2577753386 hasRelatedWork W2102312363 @default.
- W2577753386 hasRelatedWork W2107226494 @default.
- W2577753386 hasRelatedWork W2121091422 @default.
- W2577753386 hasRelatedWork W2463572558 @default.
- W2577753386 hasRelatedWork W2594063049 @default.
- W2577753386 hasRelatedWork W3015484780 @default.
- W2577753386 hasRelatedWork W303820748 @default.
- W2577753386 hasRelatedWork W3040253585 @default.
- W2577753386 hasRelatedWork W2789153098 @default.
- W2577753386 hasVolume "33" @default.
- W2577753386 isParatext "false" @default.
- W2577753386 isRetracted "false" @default.
- W2577753386 magId "2577753386" @default.
- W2577753386 workType "article" @default.