Matches in SemOpenAlex for { <https://semopenalex.org/work/W2614793576> ?p ?o ?g. }
- W2614793576 endingPage "416" @default.
- W2614793576 startingPage "398" @default.
- W2614793576 abstract "This article explores human-animal relationships in the North by calling for a fresh examination of the infrastructures and architectures which inscribe them. We draw attention to the self-limiting quality of Arctic architectures which are designed to emphasize mutual autonomy. This approach challenges models that would create a crisp, clear separation between domestication as constituting a form of domination or a type of mutualism. By describing several key infrastructures of domestication – of tethers, enclosures, and traps – we hope to draw attention to the silencing of these domestic inventories. Revisiting the metaphor of the domus, we focus on the lands where these relationships are elaborated, re-linking Arctic architectures to places of encounter. Drawing on in-depth fieldwork mainly from Northern North America and various sites in Northern Eurasia, we present an ethnographically informed account that stresses the nuanced way in which strategies of control are blended with those of care and comfort, creating unbounded homes that are good to live in. Résumé L'article explore les relations entre humains et animaux dans le Nord en invitant à réexaminer les infrastructures et les architectures qui les circonscrivent. Les auteurs attirent l'attention sur la qualité auto-délimitée des architectures arctiques, conçues pour accentuer l'autonomie mutuelle. Cette approche remet en question les modèles qui voient une séparation clairement tranchée entre la domestication comme forme de domination ou comme type de mutualisme. En décrivant plusieurs infrastructures essentielles de la domestication (longes, enclos et pièges), les auteurs espèrent attirer l'attention sur ces équipements domestiques largement oubliés. Revisitant la métaphore de la domus, ils se concentrent sur les terres oú ces relations s'élaborent, rétablissant le lien entre les architectures arctiques et les lieux de rencontre. À partir d'un travail de terrain approfondi, réalisé principalement dans le nord de l'Amérique du Nord et sur différents sites dans le nord de l'Eurasie, leur compte-rendu ethnographique souligne les diverses manières dont les stratégies de contrôle se mêlent aux stratégies de soin et de confort, créant des foyers sans démarcations où il fait bon vivre. This article explores human-animal relationships by examining the architectures inscribing them. Challenging stark dichotomies wherein domestication is seen as necessarily dominating, or at heart mutualistic, we propose an ethnographically informed account focusing on strategies of control, but also on those of care and comfort, which are delineated by evocative objects and structures. By paying attention to the material architectures of domestication – of tethers, enclosures, and traps – we highlight the silencing of domestic inventories. Rather than assuming that a single bridle dug from a Neolithic excavation, or a fenced enclosure in the taiga, ‘stand for’ the dreary domination of human over animal, we are asking readers to take a second look to imagine these artefacts as pointing to an architecture of relationships. Our examples come almost exclusively from the North – Northern North America and Northern Eurasia – and this is for several reasons. Within the regions of the world where domestication has been interrogated, there has been a surprising overemphasis on places such as the Near East, China, or the temperate zones of the Americas as so-called ‘centres of domestication’. The dominant narrative has been that domestication – as if it were a standard recipe – was achieved once in a particular place, and then spread to other parts of the world. This fairy tale of how physically distinct dogs, maize, or horses colonized the rest of the planet has been shown to be over-exaggerated, but also to have an uncanny ability to stick in people's minds as a heuristic (Gamble 2007; Terrell et al. 2003). The Arctic, by contrast, has traditionally been a place where pre-fabricated domestic forms – even if they are imported – never seem to take hold. Arctic dogs are always thought to have been bred elsewhere, to have diffused to a particular place, and then to have lost their distinctiveness by interbreeding with wolves (Lee et al. 2015; Mannermaa, Ukkonen, Kristiina & Viranta 2014; Park 1987). Similarly, reindeer and caribou, signature species of northern climates, are often only seen as ‘semi-domesticated’ (Takakura 2010; 2015), or at best objects of management, at risk of introgression (Larson & Fuller 2014: 118; Røed et al. 2014; Vigne 2011). Northern domesticates are mostly seen to be hybrids and poor copies of better examples of pure types found elsewhere. There are many reasons for this enduring anomaly. One may be the fact that, in the words of Hiroki Takakura (2010), Northern people cultivate ‘familiarity’ with all animals, wild and tame, creating the impression that Northern domestic animals are less committed, and therefore less dominated/domesticated. We would like to suggest that this perceived lack of commitment is in fact a marker of a strongly resilient style of human-animal relationship, which respects the autonomy of the domestic form. Domesticity, therefore, cannot be read off of bones and bodies – within speciation – but must be read within the environments that both animals and people respect (Hey 2006; Mallet 2008). Northern environments, in turn, are delineated by their slight but significant architectures. We do not understand architectures of domestication in the classical sense as fixed and static buildings, designed and constructed by humans alone. Such definitions separate human agency from that of others. We rather see such architectures as inscribing the activities of both humans and animals in the places where they encounter each other. The literature on domestication has enjoyed a significant revival, and that of the anthropology of domestication has been no exception (Lien 2015; Stammler & Takakura 2010; Stépanoff 2012; 2015; Willerslev, Vitebsky & Alekseyev 2015). The tone of the debate has been fundamentalist in the sense that many argue that a closer reading of human-animal interactions might help cure anthropology of its implicit evolutionary tendencies. There has been an oddly ironic quality to some of the studies. Much of the debate has been reinvigorated by the movement to identify an epoch of ecological domination known as the Anthropocene, where the dominance of sedentary and industrial forms of production is seen as both human-generated and yet strangely unavoidable (Cassidy 2012; Lövbrand et al. 2015; Moore 2016; Steffen, Grinevald, Crutzen & McNeill 2011). In Lesley Head's (2014) gentle critique of this literature, he notes that many studies deflate triumphalist narratives of progress only to retain their fatalistic unilinearism. Echoing this tendency are a recent set of post-humanist studies which argue that any relationship humans might have with animals is always dominating, and the truly liberating stance is to advocate a new set of fortress-like conservation refuges informed by philosophies of ‘edenic science’ or ‘rewilding’ (Knight 2000; Linnell, Kaczensky, Wotschikowsky, Lescureux & Boitani 2015; Robbins & Moore 2013; Tsing 2013; Wuerthner, Crist & Butler 2014). At the same time, in neighbouring laboratories, some scholars have reanalysed the genetic architecture of species commonly thought to be domestic and have come to promote complex models of mutualism and co-domestication. In these accounts, animals move between physically altered ‘domestic’ forms and ‘wild’ or feral forms without any necessary single point of origin (Larson & Fuller 2014; Vigne 2011; Zeder 2012). However, even this detailed literature often reintroduces the same directionality at a different level. A prominent recent example is Melinda Zeder's (2015) sophisticated summary where she reserves the term ‘domestication’ to apply to instances where the control of reproduction and care of one organism over another increases the mutual ‘fitness’ of both the domesticator and the partner organism. Her definition is unique and inspiring as it tracks a mutually beneficial relationship. However, it leans on older ideologies by requiring a measurable ‘documented’ change in the physical and/or genetic form of the plant or animal (Zeder, Smith, Bradley & Emshwiller 2006). The ironic quality of this debate extends to studies that are overtly critical of human exceptionalism. Although some might argue for symmetry in describing agents and material assemblages (Olsen 2010; Witmore 2014), they often unintentionally reintroduce human exceptionalism by silencing ‘the wrong type of nonhuman’ – the agency of sentient animals being a case in point (Ingold 2012: 429-31; 2013: 14-17). These studies, whether they advocate a ‘new materialism’ (Bennett 2010; Witmore 2014), a ‘new animism’ (Costa & Fausto 2010; Descola 2013), or a form of ‘ontological pluralism’ (Holbraad & Willerslev 2007; Kohn 2015), tend to advocate, perhaps unintentionally, a flat universe where the highest vantage-point is that of the isolated academic expert. Within the literature on Arctic reindeer herding societies alone, there has been a vigorous debate advocating ‘symbiotic domesticity’ in the way that reindeer and humans attend to each other (Beach & Stammler 2006; Stammler 2010). This variant of mutualism has the strong advantage of suggesting that each community in the symbiosis retains its own identity and autonomy. However, as Charles Stépanoff and his colleagues have argued recently (Stépanoff, Marchina, Fossier & Bureau 2017), this totalizing biological metaphor collapses those interesting points of tension and discontinuity sitting in between domination and mutualism which ethnographic analysis can capture. While aspects of all these debates are apt, we think that an important point has been missed by the focus on terminological and methodological puritanism. While it may be logically plausible to attribute equal agency to all things, this strict method describes a rather barren landscape where neither exceptional human interests, nor those of concerned animals (or things) break the entangled horizon. Without insisting that man is the measure of all things, it is possible to imagine a more ecological engagement with the world – where the land, things, animals, and human persons together call attention to meaningful situations. In this study, we use ethnography to focus on exactly this transitional ground – a space where more than one animal creates convivial places which are good to live in.1 There have been several studies which have pointed to the importance of understanding landscapes that tie many different species together in relationships (Anderson 2014; Berkes 2008; Johnson & Hunn 2009; Terrell et al. 2003). We argue that this locus of relations could be usefully signified by the Latin root of the term ‘domestication’: domus. The term ‘domus’ has been used in a wide variety of contexts, some of which are contradictory. Most recently, it has drawn the attention of advocates of Actor-Network Theory as a synonym for the assemblage where human-animal relationships are housed and performed (Lien 2015; Lien & Law 2016; Swanson forthcoming). It was cited prominently by Ian Hodder (1990; 1998) as a kind of compartmentalizing intuition that characterized the minds of humans during the European Neolithic. Hodder traced how prehistoric Europeans structured space within built structures only after creating symbolically separated domains of the home and the wild in their minds. His symbolic domus was criticized for the way that it exempted humans (H.M. Leach 2003: 358) and for the way it surgically extracted houses from their broader civic settings (Davis 1992). Harsher criticisms come from literary theory, where the domus is associated with a ‘bucolic’ rural ideal that underwrites violent forms of nationalism (N. Leach 1998; Lyotard 1991). The puritanical positivism of these critiques incites us to close our eyes to one type of complex environment – an environment of people, tethers, forests, animals, and glaciers – and to refocus our attention, for example, on a less diverse entanglement where there are simply more human persons milling about. By contrast, an ecological approach might focus on how the attention of many different species, and the artefacts of that attention, come to work together in a particular setting – a co-specific domus. Here there would be no need for an urban scholar to poll the interests of thousands of denizens, or a New Materialist to speak on behalf of an assemblage of things. In the introduction to an influential volume on domestication, Rebecca Cassidy and Molly Mullin (2007: 14) called for ethnographic attention to the spaces where domestication takes place. For the examples to hand, these spaces would not be Hodder's trapezoidal, roofed enclosures but a steep-sided valley, or a simple rope barrier, which subtly but effectively circumscribe Northern settings. If one were to relax the dualisms of Hodder's initial intervention, his search for long-term symbolic continuities in specific co-constructed settings would serve us well as a metaphor to describe the places where Arctic humans and animals meet. Admittedly, most European languages are lexically poor for terms which blend evocative notions of place with multispecies perception. By contrast, many circumpolar indigenous languages have terms which speak to the mutual recognition of place by multiple thinking entities. Gwich'in in Northwestern Canada speak of the landscape around them as ‘country’ – a gloss for a domus recognized by a multitude of beings (Wishart 2004: 81). In her classic ethnographic survey of the southern Yukon, Catherine McClellan (1975) adopted the creole English of the local people to describe hunting camps with the militaristic metaphor of ‘headquarters’. It is well known that Inuit naming traditions mix personal identity and landscape with the suffix -miuit (Collignon & Müller-Wille 2006). Reindeer herding Nenets have a unique gloss on the word ilebts’, which can mean both wild reindeer, life, and the dwelling – a gloss that stresses an intersection of sustenance with residence (Tereshchenko 1964). Eastern Siberian Evenki name comfortable hunting and herding places as bikit, literally as ‘a place of being’, but metaphorically as the place where the interests of humans and animals intersect (Brandišauskas 2012). The elision of being and place is sometimes better described through examples. Konstantin Klokov (2011) has developed Lev Gumilev's idea of the ‘sustaining landscape’ to describe how animal activity is wrapped together with the social and economic goals of human residents. In his detailed example of Nenets Arctic fox husbandry, he documents how Nenetses used their knowledge of how to find Arctic fox warrens with their understanding of the fox's periodic demographic explosions to encourage the animals to stay in the camp. In his account, early attempts by nomadic Nenetses to rear foxes were seen as elaborations on the knowledge required to encounter them in the places they frequent. Domesticates, too, can become landscape architects. In Northern Canada, Vuntut Gwich'in who keep working dogs describe how the dogs themselves transform the landscape of the camp. The tethered dogs create deep circular imprints on the land. These open spaces are interpreted not as artefacts of confinement but as a creative action. It is thought that the dogs keep a clear space so that they are better able to smell, see, and understand the country around them. Landscapes cultivated by animals may also include built-up, urbanized settings. Bente Sundsvold (2010; 2015), in her long-term study of North Norwegian practices of ‘bird care’ (fuglepleie), describes the way that people manage village environments to attract economically important eider ducks. The centuries-old eider down industry in Northern Norway is built around an ethic of mutual respect which placed demands on humans to modify their own behaviour, as well as to set aside built structures for the wildfowl. Properly appointed places attract the eiders such that in a particular season the human settlement itself doubles as a bird domus. One of the most passive and elliptical relations, that of fishing, also requires an intimate knowledge of being and place, including submerged geographies. Fishing entails an experimental approach: being willing to move nets and to vary their depths. Gwich'in ‘read’ the surface of the water for clues as to what lies below, and therefore the places where fish ‘feel right at home’. The river has its own architecture: its surface slows to show where the pools are, speeds up to show the riffles, boils up to show submerged obstacles, and turns back on itself in eddies. Sizeable eddies attract large numbers of fish to rest in the back-water, suggesting a good place for Gwich'in to set up fish camps – some of which remain stable over several years (Slobodin 1962: 58-9; Wishart 2014). The camp's architecture is complete when smoke houses and drying racks are set up, as well as the corresponding doggy architectures described earlier – all of which are placed in easy access of the river (Fig. 1). Emplaced relations also involve sensitivity to aerial spaces. Falconers keep an eye on atmospheric conditions when they interrelate with their birds. As with many forms of Northern domestic practice, it is crucial that falconry birds maintain a certain degree of autonomy: that they learn to fly freely, without any material constraints, while still maintaining attention to the human hunting assistant on the ground. Working together with an airborne creature, therefore, necessitates becoming attuned to birds’ way of being, always in relation to the ever-changing environmental conditions in which they are emplaced. Various forms of pastoral architecture in Eastern Siberia are built on a similar principle of not dividing animals from people but of building relationships. Vladimir Davydov (2014a; 2014b) describes reindeer herding architecture as a ‘social frame’ which intensifies the relation between people and animals. In his account, herding structures are never complete but constantly adjusted to make them fit the weather and land conditions. Among Zabaikal Evenkis, he draws attention to the ‘narrow places’ (uzkie mesta) where rocky ridges serve as natural borders to bring people and animals together. He cautions, though, that taiga dwellers, be they people or animals, are not tethered to one narrow place. Instead, they employ a patchwork of places, some of which are naturally enclosed, while others might be enhanced by a fence or a smudge structure. Reindeer architecture, then, is a form of pragmatic attention to the environment that is never separate from it. The instruments of herding, quite unlike those of hunting, are of control rather than revelation: they include the whip, spur, harness and hobble, all of them designed either to restrict or to induce movement through the infliction of physical force, and sometimes acute pain (Ingold 1994: 16-17). Ingold's inversion famously drew attention towards the subtle ways in which hunting peoples interact with the environment around them, but perhaps had the unintended consequence of turning the material attributes of pastoralism into univocal symbols of human mastery (see Knight 2012 and Oma 2010 for opposite arguments). How could hobbling or harnessing intend anything other than to restrict the horizons of a free-ranging beast? In the hands of a clumsy herder, perhaps those of an apprentice anthropologist, a harness or lasso could indeed inflict a lot of stress on an animal. However, this need not necessarily be so. Much like the classically dialogic objects of the philosophical tradition – be they Heidegger's hammer or Merleau-Ponty's blindman's cane – objects wielded properly become an extension of at least one mind signalling intention as well as, sometimes, force. The lasso can extend the reach of a herder to touch and guide the attention of a companion animal, as much as it might restrict or halt an unruly one. For example, Terhi Vuojala-Magga (2010) gives a wonderful account of how a tether both restrains and enables humans and reindeer, teaching humans and reindeer how to walk together. Similarly a falcon's hood can be employed to induce a localized environment, perhaps confining, but also calm and secure. When they work well, these become unobtrusive and assumed – what Heidegger might describe as ‘inconspicuous domination’ (Braver 2014: 45). Rather than being best seen as devices that only deliver an ultimatum, hoods and tethers are ‘boring’ infrastructures which create relationships (Star 1999). They unobtrusively help two specific personages to focus their attention towards a singular project of thriving in a particular place. In a pluriverse of noisy entities – of wind, overflying aircraft, or a creaking, fire-scarred forest – these social infrastructures help two creatures avoid distraction and concentrate on the task at hand. Small-scale taiga reindeer husbandry is a good case in point (Beach 1981: Ingold 1980; Vasilevich & Levin 1951). Within a foraging camp, the key domestic animals (reindeer, dogs, horses) are habituated to the camp but not dominated – or at least not dominated in the way that their sense of initiative and compass of movement is restricted. Although in each camp, and in each region, there is a complex yearly round, a typical snapshot from a day in the life of the herdsmen is waking, drinking tea, and then setting forth into the forest to call out for the animals and to ‘bring them back home’. Arriving at camp, which may or may not have a reindeer corral enclosure, the reindeer are given some time to rest or to forage. The herders might then walk slowly and calmly about the herd, collecting animals to harness. Some ‘learned’ well-habituated animals might allow a familiar human herder to approach and to lead that animal away. Others might have to be enticed or captured. However, even in the most extreme cases, amongst these intensively habituated herds, once a lasso is thrown and makes contact with the animal, the reindeer will often stop suddenly, not struggle, and wait patiently for the herder to approach. The lasso in these cases serves to communicate that a specific reindeer has been called out from reindeer society to participate in a particular task within the co-specific domus community. There are, of course, exceptions to this harmonious picture. Within well-habituated herds, the lasso might fall by accident on a yearling calf – a young, naïve ‘unlearned’ animal – in which case the herder has the double dilemma of how to retrieve the animal, and the lasso, without breaking the leg of the young animal. Within a taiga, now fragmented by resource extraction industries and urban spaces, there are also herds where the reindeer are not so very well habituated to their human hosts, or where the herders are frequently absent. In these cases there might be more struggle in retrieving the animals with a lasso, or indeed one might have to use a rifle to harvest a completely wild member of the herd. In these industrializing settings, the tools of reindeer husbandry appear more like the textbook cases of domination since the relation between people and reindeer has become fraught. The tools are thus employed to rebuild it. Evenki herders tend not to speak about ‘breaking’ a reindeer but rather about ‘teaching’ it. A learned reindeer is described as an animal who ‘knows the rope’ – knowing being a broad category closer to that of ‘understanding’ than to that of ‘submitting to’. In a setting where human persons and reindeer persons understand each other well, the material artefact of the tether – be it lasso, head-harness, or neck-tether – can be used to indicate direction or to focus attention on a mutual task. The tether will allow the human person to indicate a sense of setting – a microecology – for standing or being together for some time in a circumscribed landscape. The relation is not strictly symmetrical – as few things are. If in the taiga, the humans are obliged to move their camp season by season in order to accompany the reindeer to places where they are more comfortable, in the open tundra the people struggle sometimes to keep up with the animals who move on to new foraging sites every few days. In this sense, the fate of the people is tethered to the well-being of the herd. This sort of collective tethered fate is relational and metaphorical. In the archaeological literature, it has been documented and described as ‘herd following’ for North American Inupiat and Dene people, who attend to so-called ‘wild’ caribou herds (Burch & Blehr 1991). In a more colourful restatement of the same, Charles Stépanoff (2015) builds on the often-cited example of a ‘reindeer revolution’ to capture the moment when people are forced to read the instincts and ‘desires’ of the animals rather than to bend the animals to their own life-projects. Tethers sometimes connect groups of domesticates. Within Evenki forest pastoralism, there is perhaps no better example of communicative tethering than the hunting triad of dog-reindeer-human (Fig. 2). In Eastern Siberia, reindeer are kept to hand for many forest-dwellers in order to facilitate the hunt for sable. In this hunting triad, a hunter will ride atop a reindeer in the early winter with a hunting dog tethered to the side of the reindeer saddle. The dog leads the triad, tracing the scent and trail of the sable. When a sable is detected, the human releases the dog, who follows the sable's twisted trail until he discovers the animal in a tree. The human/reindeer team catches up. The human shoots the sable, sharing the meat with the dog and saving the fur. The tethers which connect the triad first allow the dog to guide the man and the reindeer, and then allow the man to guide the reindeer. Similarly, in the reindeer's mind, the tether helps him to distinguish between two settings: one where he might flee from that very same dog during the daily round-up and one where he understands that he is being called upon to perform a specific task. In Gwich'in country, the relation of animals to tethers is inverted. Dogs have generally come to be tied down in this region of the Canadian Arctic following a long history of colonial regulation by government authorities.2 Yet Gwich'in who have taken up this governmental practice do not describe it as a form of domination but rather as an accommodation for ‘keeping dogs near’. They experience it as a quality of care whereby people ensure that the dogs do not get into mischief or get preyed upon by wolves. Gwich'in dog mushers often state that the ‘dogs work for them and they work for the dogs’ (Loovers 2015). As a case in point, while training a young pup to harness, the pup is tethered to a more experienced dog so that ‘he learns to run’. Here, too, the emphasis is placed on learning and knowing the task that lies ahead. Harnessing concerns an inter-dog pedagogy as much as an interspecies human-dog relation. In the case of falconry, tethers, leashes, and the so-called ‘lure’ are essential for mediating and structuring human-bird collaboration. To interpret them simply as instruments of domination would be to eclipse the ‘tools of communication’ (Schroer 2015) that enable human and bird to become attentive to each other. Birds learn to interpret the use of various forms of tethers according to the context in which they are employed, and their meaning changes with time and growing experience. In order to secure birds, falconers use so-called ‘jesses’ – thin leather straps made from soft but durable leather – and a short leash attached to the jesses. Young, inexperienced birds are tethered most of the time while they are slowly introduced into routines. When birds are not yet familiar with their surroundings, it is said to be safest to secure them with tethers rather than leaving them unconstrained in an enclosed space in which they might harm themselves (i.e. break feathers). The act of tethering, similar to the use of the hood, becomes a framing device communicating the intentions of the falconer. Thus the tethers also enrol the humans, who will learn to discipline their bodies to be able to establish a communicative bond. Falconers often point out that it is the comfort of the bird that must come first, and it is the falconer who needs to adjust his/her movements and postures to those of the birds in order to ensure that this is the case. These examples show that the tools of domestication – and especially those in Northern settings – cannot function, or at least do not function best, when they are instruments of simply imposing the will of a human master. In the ethnographic present, as perhaps in the archaeological past, dominating forms of tethering and enclosure which might be necessary in fraught social settings should lead to more inconspicuous and habituated forms. Through circumscribing the movement of not only the animal but also the would-be dominator, they have the effect of defining a setting where the two work together. When used well, they often have an unobtrusive quality, which contributes perhaps to their invisibility in the literature. Without tethers, harnesses, and lassos, the environment would indeed dissipate action and attention such that neither animal nor human could prosper. These instruments therefore serve as social infrastructures which define the spaces where humans and animals collaborate. Beside tethers, enclosures are the most archetypically evocative set of structures that conjure a sense of control and confinement. Often built of heavy materials, and often featuring bars, they look like prisons. In the anthropological literature, enclosures occupy a privileged position. They es" @default.
- W2614793576 created "2017-05-26" @default.
- W2614793576 creator A5007024349 @default.
- W2614793576 creator A5011696390 @default.
- W2614793576 creator A5042347784 @default.
- W2614793576 creator A5060701274 @default.
- W2614793576 date "2017-05-08" @default.
- W2614793576 modified "2023-09-29" @default.
- W2614793576 title "Architectures of domestication: on emplacing human-animal relations in the North" @default.
- W2614793576 cites W1575790401 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W1608280434 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W1774817708 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W1949797249 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W1966938024 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W1985454324 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W2006693167 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W2008505800 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W2014831132 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W2014930802 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W2037683782 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W2043607791 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W2043818885 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W2049843837 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W2053388374 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W2065876140 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W2082543390 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W2082704605 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W2083406832 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W2091816113 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W2100715199 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W2102030943 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W2108883106 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W2121690840 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W2126854384 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W2135173256 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W2155208554 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W2157030269 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W2163235992 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W2164878692 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W2165082430 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W2166641504 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W2183058294 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W2191074088 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W2317290625 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W2465395915 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W2495549574 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W2580435727 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W2806635962 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W2950936092 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W4207017369 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W4229830754 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W4241954441 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W4247471499 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W4251471425 @default.
- W2614793576 cites W4254979546 @default.
- W2614793576 doi "https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9655.12613_1" @default.
- W2614793576 hasPublicationYear "2017" @default.
- W2614793576 type Work @default.
- W2614793576 sameAs 2614793576 @default.
- W2614793576 citedByCount "44" @default.
- W2614793576 countsByYear W26147935762017 @default.
- W2614793576 countsByYear W26147935762018 @default.
- W2614793576 countsByYear W26147935762019 @default.
- W2614793576 countsByYear W26147935762020 @default.
- W2614793576 countsByYear W26147935762021 @default.
- W2614793576 countsByYear W26147935762022 @default.
- W2614793576 countsByYear W26147935762023 @default.
- W2614793576 crossrefType "journal-article" @default.
- W2614793576 hasAuthorship W2614793576A5007024349 @default.
- W2614793576 hasAuthorship W2614793576A5011696390 @default.
- W2614793576 hasAuthorship W2614793576A5042347784 @default.
- W2614793576 hasAuthorship W2614793576A5060701274 @default.
- W2614793576 hasBestOaLocation W26147935761 @default.
- W2614793576 hasConcept C134387633 @default.
- W2614793576 hasConcept C138885662 @default.
- W2614793576 hasConcept C18903297 @default.
- W2614793576 hasConcept C205649164 @default.
- W2614793576 hasConcept C3017618592 @default.
- W2614793576 hasConcept C78458016 @default.
- W2614793576 hasConcept C86803240 @default.
- W2614793576 hasConcept C95124753 @default.
- W2614793576 hasConcept C95457728 @default.
- W2614793576 hasConceptScore W2614793576C134387633 @default.
- W2614793576 hasConceptScore W2614793576C138885662 @default.
- W2614793576 hasConceptScore W2614793576C18903297 @default.
- W2614793576 hasConceptScore W2614793576C205649164 @default.
- W2614793576 hasConceptScore W2614793576C3017618592 @default.
- W2614793576 hasConceptScore W2614793576C78458016 @default.
- W2614793576 hasConceptScore W2614793576C86803240 @default.
- W2614793576 hasConceptScore W2614793576C95124753 @default.
- W2614793576 hasConceptScore W2614793576C95457728 @default.
- W2614793576 hasFunder F4320334630 @default.
- W2614793576 hasIssue "2" @default.
- W2614793576 hasLocation W26147935761 @default.
- W2614793576 hasLocation W26147935762 @default.
- W2614793576 hasOpenAccess W2614793576 @default.
- W2614793576 hasPrimaryLocation W26147935761 @default.
- W2614793576 hasRelatedWork W1977837622 @default.