Matches in SemOpenAlex for { <https://semopenalex.org/work/W2619503367> ?p ?o ?g. }
Showing items 1 to 96 of
96
with 100 items per page.
- W2619503367 abstract "Frequency Trajectory Gives Rise to an Age-Limited Learning Effect as a Function of Input-Output Mapping in Connectionist Networks Martial Mermillod Patrick Bonin LAPSCO - (CNRS UMR 6024) Universite Blaise Pascal LAPSCO - (CNRS UMR 6024) Universite Blaise Pascal Tiffany Morisseau Alain Meot L2C2 - (CNRS UMR 5230) Institut des Sciences Cognitives LAPSCO - (CNRS UMR 6024) Universite Blaise Pascal Ludovic Ferrand LAPSCO - (CNRS UMR 6024) Universite Blaise Pascal Abstract According to the age of acquisition (AoA) hypothesis, words acquired early in life are processed faster and more accurately than words acquired later (see Juhasz, 2005; Johnston & Barry, 2006 for reviews). Connectionist models have begun to explore the influence of the age/order of acquisition of the items (and also their frequency of encounter) (Ellis & Lambon Ralph, 2000; Lambon Ralph & Ehsan, 2006; Zevin & Seidenberg, 2002). We explored age-limited learning effects in a connectionist model similar to that used by Lambon Ralph and Ehsan (2006) but with the use of a frequency trajectory (Zevin & Seidenberg, 2002), which refers to changes in the frequency of the words over long periods of age, since frequency trajectory is thought to better index age-limited learning effects than traditional AoA measures (Bonin, Barry, Meot, & Chalard, 2004). Our simulations show that the influence of frequency trajectory varies as a function of the mappings between input and output units in a similar type of neural network to that used by Lambon Ralph and Ehsan (2006). Introduction An important issue in psychological science is to determine whether items (words, objects, faces, etc.) which are acquired early in life are processed faster and more accurately by adults than those which are acquired later in life, namely whether there is a late influence of early acquisitions. A large number of studies have convincingly shown that words acquired early in life are processed faster and more accurately than words acquired later in life (Johnston & Barry, 2006; Juhasz, 2005 for recent reviews) using age of acquisition (AoA) norms collected from either adult ratings or from children's performance. The so-called age-of-acquisition effects have been found in a large variety of tasks (e.g., object, face and action naming, word reading, lexical decision) and in different populations (e.g., children, young and old adults, aphasics). However, despite robust AoA effects in a wide variety of lexical tasks, there is a current debate as to whether the order of acquisition of the words is per se an important factor in determining the ease of processing the words in both normal and impaired adults or whether AoA measures actually underlie other hidden factors. It is plausible that the order of acquisition of the words is a factor which is directly responsible for the ease of processing the words, and indeed this is the crucial tenet of the “AoA hypothesis”. Recent attempts to independently manipulate this factor have shown a reliable influence on the learning of artificial patterns in laboratory settings (Stewart & Ellis, 2008). However, as far as the learning of the words of a language is concerned, there are obviously, factors other than the order in which the words and/or concepts were encountered also clearly underlie the speed and accuracy of acquisition (with the result that certain words are acquired before others). These factors are truly responsible for the AoA effects found in lexical processing in adults. Among these factors are (1) the frequency of encounter of the words (e.g., during certain period of life, during one's entire life) and (2) the kind of relationships (i,e., systematic, quasi-systematic, arbitrary that exists between different types of codes (e.g., between phonological and orthographic codes, between semantic codes and phonological codes). Frequency trajectories refer to the fact that some words are more frequent during certain periods of acquisition (e.g., “dragon” during childhood) than others (e.g., “tax” during adulthood) and the words which are frequently encountered are acquired earlier than those which are encountered less frequently (Bonin, Barry, Meot, & Chalard, 2004; Hazard, De Cara, & Chanquoy, in press; Zevin & Seidenberg, 2002). But as we shall explain, the question of whether words which have been frequently encountered during a period of acquisition are easier to process later in life than words encountered less frequently also depends on the kind of relationships that exists between different types of codes (and which have to be learned). In alphabetic languages such as English or French, there are quasi-systematic relationships between sound units and orthographic units, whereas the relationships between semantic units and phonological (or orthographic) units are arbitrary. When quasi-" @default.
- W2619503367 created "2017-06-05" @default.
- W2619503367 creator A5006491133 @default.
- W2619503367 creator A5045005305 @default.
- W2619503367 creator A5065568057 @default.
- W2619503367 creator A5073695652 @default.
- W2619503367 creator A5075809821 @default.
- W2619503367 date "2009-01-01" @default.
- W2619503367 modified "2023-09-26" @default.
- W2619503367 title "Frequency Trajectory Gives Rise to an Age-Limited Learning Effect as a Function of Input-Output Mapping in Connectionist Networks" @default.
- W2619503367 cites W2023206511 @default.
- W2619503367 cites W2025192393 @default.
- W2619503367 cites W2029571693 @default.
- W2619503367 cites W2030831236 @default.
- W2619503367 cites W2036300707 @default.
- W2619503367 cites W2037683744 @default.
- W2619503367 cites W2037723377 @default.
- W2619503367 cites W2042892720 @default.
- W2619503367 cites W2046431548 @default.
- W2619503367 cites W2049252653 @default.
- W2619503367 cites W2055689961 @default.
- W2619503367 cites W2072263377 @default.
- W2619503367 cites W2076310961 @default.
- W2619503367 cites W2089235062 @default.
- W2619503367 cites W2093641773 @default.
- W2619503367 cites W2124935504 @default.
- W2619503367 cites W2135501961 @default.
- W2619503367 cites W2146538476 @default.
- W2619503367 cites W2158572527 @default.
- W2619503367 cites W2291119247 @default.
- W2619503367 cites W3207342693 @default.
- W2619503367 cites W950082919 @default.
- W2619503367 hasPublicationYear "2009" @default.
- W2619503367 type Work @default.
- W2619503367 sameAs 2619503367 @default.
- W2619503367 citedByCount "1" @default.
- W2619503367 countsByYear W26195033672013 @default.
- W2619503367 crossrefType "journal-article" @default.
- W2619503367 hasAuthorship W2619503367A5006491133 @default.
- W2619503367 hasAuthorship W2619503367A5045005305 @default.
- W2619503367 hasAuthorship W2619503367A5065568057 @default.
- W2619503367 hasAuthorship W2619503367A5073695652 @default.
- W2619503367 hasAuthorship W2619503367A5075809821 @default.
- W2619503367 hasConcept C121332964 @default.
- W2619503367 hasConcept C1276947 @default.
- W2619503367 hasConcept C13662910 @default.
- W2619503367 hasConcept C14036430 @default.
- W2619503367 hasConcept C154945302 @default.
- W2619503367 hasConcept C199360897 @default.
- W2619503367 hasConcept C41008148 @default.
- W2619503367 hasConcept C50644808 @default.
- W2619503367 hasConcept C75608658 @default.
- W2619503367 hasConcept C78458016 @default.
- W2619503367 hasConcept C8521452 @default.
- W2619503367 hasConcept C86803240 @default.
- W2619503367 hasConceptScore W2619503367C121332964 @default.
- W2619503367 hasConceptScore W2619503367C1276947 @default.
- W2619503367 hasConceptScore W2619503367C13662910 @default.
- W2619503367 hasConceptScore W2619503367C14036430 @default.
- W2619503367 hasConceptScore W2619503367C154945302 @default.
- W2619503367 hasConceptScore W2619503367C199360897 @default.
- W2619503367 hasConceptScore W2619503367C41008148 @default.
- W2619503367 hasConceptScore W2619503367C50644808 @default.
- W2619503367 hasConceptScore W2619503367C75608658 @default.
- W2619503367 hasConceptScore W2619503367C78458016 @default.
- W2619503367 hasConceptScore W2619503367C8521452 @default.
- W2619503367 hasConceptScore W2619503367C86803240 @default.
- W2619503367 hasIssue "31" @default.
- W2619503367 hasLocation W26195033671 @default.
- W2619503367 hasOpenAccess W2619503367 @default.
- W2619503367 hasPrimaryLocation W26195033671 @default.
- W2619503367 hasRelatedWork W1483645496 @default.
- W2619503367 hasRelatedWork W1979185136 @default.
- W2619503367 hasRelatedWork W1989635369 @default.
- W2619503367 hasRelatedWork W2046049927 @default.
- W2619503367 hasRelatedWork W2061797559 @default.
- W2619503367 hasRelatedWork W2079218657 @default.
- W2619503367 hasRelatedWork W2080816533 @default.
- W2619503367 hasRelatedWork W2157564630 @default.
- W2619503367 hasRelatedWork W2166265637 @default.
- W2619503367 hasRelatedWork W2185440720 @default.
- W2619503367 hasRelatedWork W225120028 @default.
- W2619503367 hasRelatedWork W2413627333 @default.
- W2619503367 hasRelatedWork W2562557088 @default.
- W2619503367 hasRelatedWork W2587135168 @default.
- W2619503367 hasRelatedWork W2588326892 @default.
- W2619503367 hasRelatedWork W2590391207 @default.
- W2619503367 hasRelatedWork W2591107532 @default.
- W2619503367 hasRelatedWork W2621060234 @default.
- W2619503367 hasRelatedWork W2767453073 @default.
- W2619503367 hasRelatedWork W772764704 @default.
- W2619503367 hasVolume "31" @default.
- W2619503367 isParatext "false" @default.
- W2619503367 isRetracted "false" @default.
- W2619503367 magId "2619503367" @default.
- W2619503367 workType "article" @default.