Matches in SemOpenAlex for { <https://semopenalex.org/work/W2890891213> ?p ?o ?g. }
- W2890891213 abstract "Language dominance is a multidimensional construct comprising several distinct yet interrelated components, including language proficiency, exposure and use. The exact relation between these components remains unclear. Several studies have observed a (non-linear) relationship between bilingual children’s amount of exposure and absolute proficiency in each language, but our understanding of the relationship between language exposure and use and relative proficiency is limited. To address this question, we examined whether experiential-based measures of language dominance, operationalised here in the narrow sense of relative language proficiency, can provide an efficient alternative to the more labour-intensive performance-based measures often used in the literature. In earlier work, Unsworth (2016a) examined the relationship between relative proficiency and language exposure and use in a group of English-Dutch bilingual preschool children residing in the Netherlands. This study expands these findings by examining Dutch-English preschool children of the same age residing in the UK in order to cover the full dominance continuum. Participants were 35 simultaneous bilingual children (2;0-5;0) exposed to English and Dutch, 20 resident in the Netherlands and 15 in the UK. Relative amount of language exposure and use were estimated using a parental questionnaire. To obtain performance-based measures of language proficiency, children’s spontaneous speech was recorded during a half-hour play session in each language. The transcribed data were used to derive MLU (words), average length of the longest five utterances, the number of different verb and noun types. Single word vocabulary comprehension was assessed using standardised tests in both languages. Following Yip and Matthews (2006), relative proficiency was operationalised using differentials. In line with Unsworth (2016a), English-dominant children typically had less than approx. 35% exposure to Dutch and used Dutch less than approx. 30% of the time. Curve-fitting analyses revealed that non-linear models best fit the data. Logistic regression analyses showed that both exposure and use were good predictors of dominance group membership assigned using the same approach as Unsworth (2016a), that is, using SDs. Dominance groups derived independently using cluster analyses overlapped with the groups derived using SDs, confirming that relative amount of exposure and use can be used as a proxy for language dominance." @default.
- W2890891213 created "2018-09-27" @default.
- W2890891213 creator A5023908741 @default.
- W2890891213 creator A5045029612 @default.
- W2890891213 creator A5062981168 @default.
- W2890891213 date "2018-10-17" @default.
- W2890891213 modified "2023-10-16" @default.
- W2890891213 title "Experiential Measures Can Be Used as a Proxy for Language Dominance in Bilingual Language Acquisition Research" @default.
- W2890891213 cites W1574328445 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W1975285189 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W1981794329 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W1985601219 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W1992327600 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W1996714330 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W1998143444 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W2001399633 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W2011046664 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W2016381774 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W2027266261 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W2059638013 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W2067372308 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W2068730462 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W2078129874 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W2090093550 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W2093189634 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W2097203685 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W2100060407 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W2108417568 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W2109227335 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W2125945489 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W2128372969 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W2139751367 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W2142716289 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W2147521188 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W2151763739 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W2157329169 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W2159681604 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W2162913043 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W2167092954 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W2168171445 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W2254666873 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W2322934038 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W2385838428 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W2414895359 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W2470645072 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W2479657989 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W2495656054 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W2501514862 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W2593529919 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W2799697499 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W2803550459 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W3125937743 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W4214739450 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W4231826137 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W4245198908 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W4294141750 @default.
- W2890891213 cites W4300009529 @default.
- W2890891213 doi "https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01809" @default.
- W2890891213 hasPubMedCentralId "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/6199388" @default.
- W2890891213 hasPubMedId "https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/30386273" @default.
- W2890891213 hasPublicationYear "2018" @default.
- W2890891213 type Work @default.
- W2890891213 sameAs 2890891213 @default.
- W2890891213 citedByCount "33" @default.
- W2890891213 countsByYear W28908912132018 @default.
- W2890891213 countsByYear W28908912132019 @default.
- W2890891213 countsByYear W28908912132020 @default.
- W2890891213 countsByYear W28908912132021 @default.
- W2890891213 countsByYear W28908912132022 @default.
- W2890891213 countsByYear W28908912132023 @default.
- W2890891213 crossrefType "journal-article" @default.
- W2890891213 hasAuthorship W2890891213A5023908741 @default.
- W2890891213 hasAuthorship W2890891213A5045029612 @default.
- W2890891213 hasAuthorship W2890891213A5062981168 @default.
- W2890891213 hasBestOaLocation W28908912131 @default.
- W2890891213 hasConcept C104317684 @default.
- W2890891213 hasConcept C138496976 @default.
- W2890891213 hasConcept C138885662 @default.
- W2890891213 hasConcept C145420912 @default.
- W2890891213 hasConcept C151913843 @default.
- W2890891213 hasConcept C15744967 @default.
- W2890891213 hasConcept C166007726 @default.
- W2890891213 hasConcept C169760540 @default.
- W2890891213 hasConcept C185592680 @default.
- W2890891213 hasConcept C2777601683 @default.
- W2890891213 hasConcept C2778883600 @default.
- W2890891213 hasConcept C41895202 @default.
- W2890891213 hasConcept C49876356 @default.
- W2890891213 hasConcept C511192102 @default.
- W2890891213 hasConcept C55493867 @default.
- W2890891213 hasConceptScore W2890891213C104317684 @default.
- W2890891213 hasConceptScore W2890891213C138496976 @default.
- W2890891213 hasConceptScore W2890891213C138885662 @default.
- W2890891213 hasConceptScore W2890891213C145420912 @default.
- W2890891213 hasConceptScore W2890891213C151913843 @default.
- W2890891213 hasConceptScore W2890891213C15744967 @default.
- W2890891213 hasConceptScore W2890891213C166007726 @default.
- W2890891213 hasConceptScore W2890891213C169760540 @default.
- W2890891213 hasConceptScore W2890891213C185592680 @default.
- W2890891213 hasConceptScore W2890891213C2777601683 @default.