Matches in SemOpenAlex for { <https://semopenalex.org/work/W3138946683> ?p ?o ?g. }
Showing items 1 to 70 of
70
with 100 items per page.
- W3138946683 endingPage "436" @default.
- W3138946683 startingPage "434" @default.
- W3138946683 abstract "We propose a framework in which top-down inhibitory control networks are impaired by sleep deprivation, giving rise to intrusive thoughts and, consequently, emotion dysregulation. This process leads to a vicious cycle of sleeplessness, persistent unwanted thoughts, and heightened anxiety, ultimately increasing the risk of mental illness. We propose a framework in which top-down inhibitory control networks are impaired by sleep deprivation, giving rise to intrusive thoughts and, consequently, emotion dysregulation. This process leads to a vicious cycle of sleeplessness, persistent unwanted thoughts, and heightened anxiety, ultimately increasing the risk of mental illness. Intrusive thoughts occur when everyday encounters trigger the retrieval of unpleasant past experiences. One way that people control intrusive thoughts is by directly suppressing them, thereby relieving consciousness of unwanted memories. Beyond this initial respite, memory suppression has far-reaching benefits. Specifically, suppressing an unwanted thought weakens the corresponding memory trace, reducing the likelihood of recurrent thought intrusions [1.Benoit R.G. et al.Adaptive top–down suppression of hippocampal activity and the purging of intrusive memories from consciousness.J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2015; 27: 96-111Crossref PubMed Scopus (87) Google Scholar]. Moreover, memory suppression alleviates the affective charge associated with unpleasant past experiences, dampening the emotional intensity of aversive memories [2.Gagnepain P. et al.Parallel regulation of memory and emotion supports the suppression of intrusive memories.J. Neurosci. 2017; 37: 6423-6441Crossref PubMed Scopus (82) Google Scholar]. These lasting benefits of memory suppression are believed to be integral to healthy emotion regulation [3.Engen H.G. Anderson M.C. Memory control: a fundamental mechanism of emotion regulation.Trends Cogn. Sci. 2018; 22: 982-995Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (75) Google Scholar]. Memory suppression is orchestrated by the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (rDLPFC), which, via top-down inhibitory mechanisms, downregulates retrieval-related activity in hippocampus and arousal responses in amygdala [2.Gagnepain P. et al.Parallel regulation of memory and emotion supports the suppression of intrusive memories.J. Neurosci. 2017; 37: 6423-6441Crossref PubMed Scopus (82) Google Scholar]. We propose a framework in which the inhibitory control network underpinning memory suppression is impaired by sleep loss. This impairment allows intrusive thoughts to break into consciousness unabated, undermining the benefits of memory suppression for emotion regulation. We argue that deficient memory control plays an important role in the well-established relationship between sleep disturbance and anxiety disorder. Central to our framework is that sleep loss diminishes people’s ability to suppress unwanted thoughts by impairing top-down inhibitory projections from rDLPFC to hippocampus and amygdala. There are several lines of evidence to support this idea. The rDLPFC is a domain-general executive control region that is recruited to suppress motor responses as well as unwanted thoughts [4.Apšvalka D. et al.Dynamic targeting enables domain-general inhibitory control over action and thought by the prefrontal cortex.bioRxiv. 2020; (Published online October 24, 2020. https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.10.22.350520)Google Scholar]. Sleep deprivation leads to reduced rDLPFC activation during motor response inhibition, resulting in poorer inhibitory control [5.Zhao R. et al.Decreased cortical and subcortical response to inhibition control after sleep deprivation.Brain Imaging Behav. 2019; 13: 638-650Crossref PubMed Scopus (19) Google Scholar]. Correspondingly, suppressing unwanted thoughts is more difficult under conditions of mental fatigue [6.van Schie K. Anderson M.C. Successfully controlling intrusive memories is harder when control must be sustained.Memory. 2017; 25: 1201-1216Crossref PubMed Scopus (26) Google Scholar]. Recent work has demonstrated that sleep-deprived individuals are ineffective at suppressing unwanted thoughts [7.Harrington M.O. et al.Losing control: sleep deprivation impairs the suppression of unwanted thoughts.Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2021; 9: 97-113Crossref PubMed Scopus (13) Google Scholar]. Whereas memory suppression reduces future thought intrusions in well-rested people, sleep-deprived individuals fail to exhibit the same intrusion-reducing benefit of effective memory control. Moreover, even after sleep-deprived individuals gain command of an unwanted thought, they are prone to relapses in memory control, such that the previously suppressed thought re-emerges into conscious awareness. These empirical findings collectively support the mainstay of our framework: sleep deprivation impairs the inhibitory control network underlying memory suppression, allowing unwanted thoughts to prevail over futile suppression attempts and remain intrusive over time. Emotion dysregulation is a robust consequence of insufficient sleep. Another component of our framework is that emotional disturbances following sleep loss arise because of impaired memory control. This idea builds on the view that memory control constitutes a fundamental mechanism of emotion regulation [3.Engen H.G. Anderson M.C. Memory control: a fundamental mechanism of emotion regulation.Trends Cogn. Sci. 2018; 22: 982-995Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (75) Google Scholar]. From this perspective, by evoking retrieval of an unwanted memory, seemingly innocuous stimuli have the power to elicit emotional reactions that mimic the original, traumatic experience. Hence, failure to suppress an unwanted memory when sleep deprived allows both the content and affective tone of that memory (represented by hippocampus and amygdala, respectively [2.Gagnepain P. et al.Parallel regulation of memory and emotion supports the suppression of intrusive memories.J. Neurosci. 2017; 37: 6423-6441Crossref PubMed Scopus (82) Google Scholar]) to intrude into consciousness, prompting a deterioration of emotional well-being. Supporting this view, whereas in well-rested people repeatedly suppressing thoughts about unpleasant images reduces subjective and psychophysiological arousal responses to those images, no such salutary benefit of memory suppression is observed in sleep-deprived individuals [7.Harrington M.O. et al.Losing control: sleep deprivation impairs the suppression of unwanted thoughts.Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2021; 9: 97-113Crossref PubMed Scopus (13) Google Scholar]. Our framework can be extended to exogenously generated emotions, which are also strongly influenced by memory (e.g., aversive responses to snakes are derived from learned expectations about the dangers posed by snakes [3.Engen H.G. Anderson M.C. Memory control: a fundamental mechanism of emotion regulation.Trends Cogn. Sci. 2018; 22: 982-995Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (75) Google Scholar]). Accordingly, failure to effectively downregulate pre-existing representations of fear after sleep loss will lead to hyperarousal when encountering a potential threat. Indeed, sleep deprivation impairs inhibitory connections from prefrontal cortex to amygdala during exposure to aversive video clips, leading to hyperactivity in amygdala and heightened feelings of anxiety [8.Ben Simon E. et al.Overanxious and underslept.Nat. Hum. Behav. 2020; 4: 100-110Crossref PubMed Scopus (59) Google Scholar]. Together, these data support the second key principle of our framework: impaired memory control following sleep loss undermines emotion regulation by allowing the affective charge of unpleasant experiences to endure. We argue that the well-known relationship between insomnia and anxiety disorder is underpinned, in part, by impaired memory control. Under our framework, deficiency of the memory suppression network following bouts of insufficient sleep gives rise to intrusive thoughts and, consequently, emotion dysregulation. Incessant thought intrusions and affective disturbances exacerbate sleep problems, leading to accumulating sleep debt, more persistent and distressing intrusive thoughts, and amplified emotional distress. This vicious cycle (Figure 1) culminates in a chronic and debilitating state of anxiety. In the weeks and months following a traumatic experience, many individuals will overcome the adversity unscathed. Others, however, will go on to develop post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), an anxiety disorder characterized by intrusive and distressing thoughts about the traumatic event. Given the prevalence of chronic sleep disturbances among PTSD sufferers, our framework can offer novel insights into how PTSD may develop in the aftermath of trauma. Specifically, deficiencies in the memory control network following sleep loss impair attempts to quash thoughts about a distressing event, preventing downregulation of the traumatic memory trace and its emotional charge. Failed suppression may also strengthen memories in a manner akin to retrieval practice, rendering them more resilient to future suppression attempts. Consistent with this view, among survivors of the 2015 Paris terrorist attacks, those who went on to develop PTSD exhibited disruption in memory control networks during attempts to suppress unwanted thoughts [9.Mary A. et al.Resilience after trauma: the role of memory suppression.Science. 2020; 367eaay8477Crossref PubMed Scopus (79) Google Scholar]. Correspondingly, whereas repeatedly suppressing specific memories weakens those memories in healthy, trauma-exposed individuals, PTSD sufferers fail to achieve the same suppression-induced forgetting benefit [10.Catarino A. et al.Failing to forget: inhibitory-control deficits compromise memory suppression in posttraumatic stress disorder.Psychol. Sci. 2015; 26: 604-616Crossref PubMed Scopus (115) Google Scholar]. Note that our framework speaks to traumatic memories that are already consolidated. Whether sleep loss in the immediate aftermath of trauma (which might arise from upsetting memories of the traumatic event itself) could help to prevent PTSD by disrupting overnight consolidation is a separate issue. The self-reinforcing cycle of disturbed sleep, intrusive thoughts, and emotion dysregulation that we have described can also be applied to other psychiatric disorders. Specifically, our framework can provide insights into the pathology of major depressive disorder, obsessive-compulsive disorder, and schizophrenia, all of which are characterized by persistent, unwanted thoughts and co-occurring sleep disturbances. Insomnia is nevertheless associated with biological irregularities that strongly impact the response to stress (i.e., dysregulation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis), potentially heightening anxiety risk in a manner that is distinct from the proposed memory control deficits arising from acute sleep deprivation. As an alternative to memory suppression, effective memory control can be achieved via thought substitution, which involves redirecting retrieval operations to other, nonaversive memories. The brain mechanisms of thought substitution are distinct from those underpinning memory suppression, relying on a predominantly left-lateralized network centered on the caudal and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex [11.Benoit R.G. Anderson M.C. Opposing mechanisms support the voluntary forgetting of unwanted memories.Neuron. 2012; 76: 450-460Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (204) Google Scholar]. Given these opposing mechanisms, the consequences of sleep deprivation might be less severe for thought substitution than for memory suppression, although the efficacy of thought substitution among individuals with psychiatric disorders (for whom sleep disturbances are commonplace) is an open issue at present [12.Stramaccia D.F. et al.Memory suppression and its deficiency in psychological disorders: a focused meta-analysis.J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2020; (Published online October 22, 2020. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000971)Crossref PubMed Scopus (18) Google Scholar]. We have outlined a neurocognitive framework to explain how sleep loss gives rise to intrusive thoughts and emotion dysregulation. We propose that sleep deprivation impairs top-down inhibitory control of brain regions involved in memory retrieval and emotional arousal. The consequences of this impairment are twofold. First, unwelcome thoughts are given free rein to dominate awareness. Second, the abiding benefits of memory suppression are bypassed, allowing aversive memories and their emotional tone to endure. This process is self-reinforcing and can contribute to the development of anxiety disorder. Testable predictions for our framework are outlined in Box 1. We expect that impairment of memory control mechanisms will emerge as a key contributor to the link between sleep disturbances and mental illness.Box 1Predictions for Future Research•Does sleep loss impair top-down inhibitory control of brain regions involved in memory retrieval and emotional arousal? Sleep deprivation should weaken inhibitory projections from rDLPFC to hippocampus and amygdala during memory suppression.•Can the well-known link between sleep loss and emotion dysregulation be attributed to deficient control of aversive memories? The extent to which memory suppression networks are impaired by sleep deprivation should predict the degree of emotional disturbance.•Do the effects of sleep loss on memory suppression abilities predict resilience to trauma? Individuals who exhibit robust memory control when sleep deprived should be least likely to develop PTSD following exposure to trauma.•How is thought substitution affected by sleep loss? Sleep-deprived individuals might be able to achieve effective memory control by directing retrieval operations to other, nonaversive memories. •Does sleep loss impair top-down inhibitory control of brain regions involved in memory retrieval and emotional arousal? Sleep deprivation should weaken inhibitory projections from rDLPFC to hippocampus and amygdala during memory suppression.•Can the well-known link between sleep loss and emotion dysregulation be attributed to deficient control of aversive memories? The extent to which memory suppression networks are impaired by sleep deprivation should predict the degree of emotional disturbance.•Do the effects of sleep loss on memory suppression abilities predict resilience to trauma? Individuals who exhibit robust memory control when sleep deprived should be least likely to develop PTSD following exposure to trauma.•How is thought substitution affected by sleep loss? Sleep-deprived individuals might be able to achieve effective memory control by directing retrieval operations to other, nonaversive memories. This work was supported by a Medical Research Council Career Development Award (MR/P020208/1) to S.A.C. We are grateful to three anonymous reviewers for their invaluable comments and suggestions on an earlier version of this paper. No interests are declared." @default.
- W3138946683 created "2021-03-29" @default.
- W3138946683 creator A5071690413 @default.
- W3138946683 creator A5083803014 @default.
- W3138946683 date "2021-06-01" @default.
- W3138946683 modified "2023-10-16" @default.
- W3138946683 title "Sleep Loss Gives Rise to Intrusive Thoughts" @default.
- W3138946683 cites W2112823205 @default.
- W3138946683 cites W2148568320 @default.
- W3138946683 cites W2155998491 @default.
- W3138946683 cites W2589828316 @default.
- W3138946683 cites W2616873190 @default.
- W3138946683 cites W2800627081 @default.
- W3138946683 cites W2887824124 @default.
- W3138946683 cites W2962477339 @default.
- W3138946683 cites W2987569059 @default.
- W3138946683 cites W2995474347 @default.
- W3138946683 cites W3005948235 @default.
- W3138946683 doi "https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2021.03.001" @default.
- W3138946683 hasPubMedId "https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/33727016" @default.
- W3138946683 hasPublicationYear "2021" @default.
- W3138946683 type Work @default.
- W3138946683 sameAs 3138946683 @default.
- W3138946683 citedByCount "12" @default.
- W3138946683 countsByYear W31389466832021 @default.
- W3138946683 countsByYear W31389466832022 @default.
- W3138946683 countsByYear W31389466832023 @default.
- W3138946683 crossrefType "journal-article" @default.
- W3138946683 hasAuthorship W3138946683A5071690413 @default.
- W3138946683 hasAuthorship W3138946683A5083803014 @default.
- W3138946683 hasBestOaLocation W31389466832 @default.
- W3138946683 hasConcept C11171543 @default.
- W3138946683 hasConcept C111919701 @default.
- W3138946683 hasConcept C15744967 @default.
- W3138946683 hasConcept C180747234 @default.
- W3138946683 hasConcept C188147891 @default.
- W3138946683 hasConcept C2775841894 @default.
- W3138946683 hasConcept C41008148 @default.
- W3138946683 hasConceptScore W3138946683C11171543 @default.
- W3138946683 hasConceptScore W3138946683C111919701 @default.
- W3138946683 hasConceptScore W3138946683C15744967 @default.
- W3138946683 hasConceptScore W3138946683C180747234 @default.
- W3138946683 hasConceptScore W3138946683C188147891 @default.
- W3138946683 hasConceptScore W3138946683C2775841894 @default.
- W3138946683 hasConceptScore W3138946683C41008148 @default.
- W3138946683 hasFunder F4320334626 @default.
- W3138946683 hasIssue "6" @default.
- W3138946683 hasLocation W31389466831 @default.
- W3138946683 hasLocation W31389466832 @default.
- W3138946683 hasLocation W31389466833 @default.
- W3138946683 hasLocation W31389466834 @default.
- W3138946683 hasOpenAccess W3138946683 @default.
- W3138946683 hasPrimaryLocation W31389466831 @default.
- W3138946683 hasRelatedWork W2053487507 @default.
- W3138946683 hasRelatedWork W2067108088 @default.
- W3138946683 hasRelatedWork W2077865380 @default.
- W3138946683 hasRelatedWork W2083375246 @default.
- W3138946683 hasRelatedWork W2085372204 @default.
- W3138946683 hasRelatedWork W2134894512 @default.
- W3138946683 hasRelatedWork W2748952813 @default.
- W3138946683 hasRelatedWork W2765597752 @default.
- W3138946683 hasRelatedWork W2899084033 @default.
- W3138946683 hasRelatedWork W2931662336 @default.
- W3138946683 hasVolume "25" @default.
- W3138946683 isParatext "false" @default.
- W3138946683 isRetracted "false" @default.
- W3138946683 magId "3138946683" @default.
- W3138946683 workType "article" @default.