Matches in SemOpenAlex for { <https://semopenalex.org/work/W799073868> ?p ?o ?g. }
Showing items 1 to 75 of
75
with 100 items per page.
- W799073868 endingPage "28" @default.
- W799073868 startingPage "10" @default.
- W799073868 abstract "You have accessThe ASHA LeaderFeature1 Aug 2004Boosting Memory With Informational CounselingHelping Patients Understand the Nature of Disorders and How to Manage Them Robert H. Margolis Robert H. Margolis Google Scholar More articles by this author https://doi.org/10.1044/leader.FTR5.09142004.10 SectionsAbout ToolsAdd to favorites ShareFacebookTwitterLinked In Our patients lead busy lives and many factors work against the likelihood that they will remember what we tell them. Informational counseling is intended to provide the patient with the relevant knowledge necessary to understand the nature of the disorder and the steps recommended for managing it. Personal adjustment counseling helps the patient and family deal with the emotional impact of the information. Both are necessary. The communications professions, however, have paid little attention to informational counseling and to our patients’ memory for the critical information that they need to play an active, positive role in remediation and rehabilitation. Although patient recall has been studied by other health professions, our literature contains almost no references to these studies. Patient Recall The working mom whose son broke an ankle yesterday playing soccer, who is worried about missing work, who doesn’t have anything for dinner tonight, and whose husband may be laid off next week, is not likely to remember important details about a communication disorder. We seldom know the complexities of our patients’ lives and their ability to comprehend and retain important information. But retention of diagnostic information and recommendations are critical to treatment outcomes. Studies have found that when patients understand the information communicated by a health care provider, it results in significant enhancements of patient satisfaction, compliance with recommendations, and outcomes, and decreases in anxiety, treatment time, and cost (e.g., Thomson, Cunningham, & Hunt 2001). One study showed that physicians underestimate their patients’ desire for information and their ability to understand medical findings (Shapiro et al., 1992). When physicians were given specific strategies for enhancing communication, patient recall improved measurably (Ley, 1977). A disturbing finding, however, is that what physicians thought patients would remember did not correlate with measures of actual recall (Anderson et al., 1979). This reinforces the need to provide information in writing, even for patients who appear to be absorbing everything. Limits On Memory Patients retain about 50% of information provided by health care providers (e.g. Shapiro et al., 1992). Depending on conditions, 40–80% may be forgotten immediately (Kessels, 2003). When recall was measured at two points, there were no differences in recall measured soon after the consultation and at a later date (Joyce et al., 1969; Reynolds et al., 1981). It seems that patients remember a small proportion of facts and those stay with them for a period of at least several weeks. Of the information that is recalled, about half is remembered incorrectly (Anderson et al., 1979; Kessels, 2003). So half is forgotten immediately, and half of that is wrong. If you remove 50% of the facts relating to a health problem and distort half the remaining information, the result can be a dangerously misunderstood message that could have life-threatening consequences. An even more disturbing finding is that patients often forget their medical diagnoses. In one study, patients could not recall 68% of the diagnoses told to them in a medical visit. When there were multiple diagnoses, patients could not recall the most important diagnosis 54% of the time (Scheitel et al., 1996). Some of the diagnoses were serious, even life-threatening conditions such as diabetes, hypertension, and liver disease. In another study, patients and physicians agreed on problems that required follow-up for only 45% of the time (Starfield et al., 1979). When there was disagreement between the physician and patient regarding the need for follow-up, the likelihood of appropriate management was significantly lower. Factors Affecting Patient Recall Patient Factors. Intelligence does not affect the ability to retain information (Ley, 1979). However, familiarity with the information does have an effect (Tuckett et al., 1985). A patient who is familiar with hearing loss as a result of prior consultations, an affected family member, or professional knowledge tends to remember more. The degree of understanding of issues related to the diagnosis can have a significant effect. A finding that the patient expects is remembered more than one that is unexpected; a finding that is welcome or desired is more likely to be recalled than one that is unwelcome or unwanted. Interestingly, patients are better able to recall information when they are in the same emotional and physical state they were in when they received the information (Kessels, 2003). Elderly patients tend to remember less than younger patients. Anxiety can have a positive or negative effect on retention. Moderate anxiety enhances recall but severe anxiety inhibits retention of information. Stress causes “attention narrowing” that interferes with the patient’s ability to redirect to a different topic (Kessels, 2003). Denial is a powerful defense mechanism that can interfere with recall of the most obvious findings. One study, for example, showed that patients frequently forgot diagnoses of excessive tobacco use and obesity (Scheitel et al., 1996). Patients who are in denial of their hearing loss, for example, are not likely to accurately convey information provided at the hearing evaluation to family members. Mode of Presentation. Information presented in a simple, easy-to-understand format is remembered better than information presented in a more complex manner. The more information presented, the lower the proportion that is recalled by the patient. Information that is presented first tends to be remembered better-the primacy effect. Categorizing information can improve retention; some authors discuss the method of explicit categorization (Kessels, 2003; Ley 1977, 1979; Tuckett et al., 1985). Information is organized in specific categories such as explanation of systems, diagnostic tests, results, prognosis, and recommendations. The patient is told that the information will be presented in these categories, each category is announced, and the patient is asked if there are questions before moving on to the next category. The method can significantly enhance recall. When verbal presentation is supplemented by written and graphical material such as written explanations, cartoons, and pictures, recall can be significantly enhanced. Recommendations are more likely to be remembered and followed when they are specific rather than general. A recommendation should be a specific statement telling the patient what to do rather than a more general statement of the goal. A recommendation to “stay home from work and rest for two weeks with no strenuous exercise” is more likely to be followed than “get some rest and take it easy for a while.” Clinician Factors. The clinician’s communication style can have a significant influence on retention. Information given by clinicians who speak in clear language with simple sentence structure is more likely to be remembered than that provided in complex, scientific language. Clarity of communication requires the clinician to understand what patients wish to learn and their levels of understanding. To communicate clearly in a manner that promotes retention of information, the consultation needs to be a dialogue in which the clinician listens to the patient. When the patient’s ideas are evaded or inhibited, the patient is less likely to remember important information. Even the clinician’s anxiety affects recall. Patients remember less when the information is provided by an overtly anxious clinician. Information that is presented in a manner that emphasizes its importance is more likely to be remembered than if it is presented in a matter-of-fact manner. Non-verbal communication is important in reflecting the clinician’s state (confident, anxious, distracted, empathetic) and in indicating the importance of information. A patient may appear to understand but if the understanding is not confirmed by the clinician and the patient is not encouraged to ask questions, the clinician may not be aware that the patient did not understand at all. Information that is unorganized, unclear, or incomplete can be interpreted by patients to confirm their pre-existing beliefs that may not be in concert with the message the clinician is attempting to communicate. Tuckett and colleagues (1985) called this the “illusion of shared understanding.” When the clinician is oblivious to the patient’s lack of understanding, the entire consultation session may provide little benefit, or worse, do more harm than good. Accurate recall can be significantly enhanced by proper communication techniques (see sidebar, page 11). But patients still will forget. The best way to ensure that the information gets home is to provide the patient with a permanent record. It is recommended that patients be instructed to write the information as the clinician presents it. Starfield et al. (1979) recommended tape-recording the consultation. Another approach is to provide clearly written, illustrated, patient-specific educational materials that ensure the information is clear, accurate, complete, and available for review and discussion with family members and other professionals. Patients cannot always bring a family member or friend to a consultation and at the time of the consultation they may not be receptive to important information. They may have prior beliefs that may or may not be realistic or they may be in denial. The research findings discussed in this article indicate that the information we present will not be remembered accurately. If the information is not remembered accurately, we cannot expect the patient to communicate accurately to family members. Although the professions of audiology and speech-language pathology are solely concerned with the communicative well-being of our patients, our own communication to patients is disordered. We complain that our counseling efforts are not reimbursed, but an analysis of our methods and outcomes would probably not convince payers that we are providing a valuable, reimbursable service when we verbally present complex information in a format that is known to be ineffective. I recommend the following guiding principle for our communication of results and recommendations to our patients: Any information that is important for the patient to understand and remember should be provided in writing. Methods of Maximizing Retention In giving advice, use concrete instructions. “Use ear plugs when you use your power tools” is more effective thank “Keep noise exposure to a minimum.” Use easy-to-understand language, with short words and sentences. Present the most important information first (the primacy effect). Often the most important information is a recommendation such as “make an appointment with the ear doctor.” Stress the importance of recommendations or other information that you want the patient to remember. Use the method of explicit categorization. Tell the patient “We are going to go over recommendations, then we will talk about how your hearing problem (diagnosis), then we will go over test results, then we will talk about how your hearing may change in the future (prognosis).” Ask for questions before moving on to the next category. Repeat the most important information. Don’t present too much information. Present only what the patient needs to remember. Be sure you understand what patients want from the evaluation and what their beliefs are concerning the problem. Specifically address the patient’s desires and beliefs. Supplement verbal information with written, graphical, and pictorial materials that the patient can take home. literature on Patient Recall References Anderson J. L., Dodman S., Kopelman M., & Fleming A. (1979). Patient information recall in a rheumatology clinic.Rheumatology and Rehabililtation,, 18, 18–22. CrossrefGoogle Scholar Bailey H. A. T., & Martin F. N. (1961). Letter to the patient with a sensori-neural hearing loss.Laryngoscope, 71, 38. CrossrefGoogle Scholar Bradshaw P. W., Ley P., & Kincey J. A. (1975). Recall of medical advice: comprehensibility and specificity.British Journal of Clinical Psychology,, 14, 55–82. CrossrefGoogle Scholar Godwin Y. (2000). Do they listen? A review of information retained by patients following consent for reduction mammoplasty.British Journal of Plastic Surgery,, 53, 121–125. CrossrefGoogle Scholar Houts P. S., Bachrach R., Witmer J. T., Tringali C. A., Bucher J. A., & Localio R. A. (1998). Using pictographs to enhance recall of spoken medical instructions.Patient Education and Counseling,, 35, 83–88. CrossrefGoogle Scholar Houts P. S., Witmer J. T., Egeth H. E., Loscalzo M. J., & Zabora J. R. (2001). Using pictographs to enhance recall of spoken medical instructions. II.Patient Education and Counseling,, 43, 231–242. CrossrefGoogle Scholar Joyce C. R. B., Caple G., Mason M., Reynolds E., & Mathews J. A. (1969). Quantitative study of doctor-patient communication.Quarterly Journal of Medicine, 38, 183–194. Google Scholar Kessels R. P. C. (2003). Patients’ memory for medical information.Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine,, 96, 219–222. Google Scholar Ley P. (1972). Primacy, rated importance, and the recall of medical statements.Journal of Health & Social Behavior,, 13, 311–317. CrossrefGoogle Scholar Ley P. (1977). In Rachman S. (Ed.), Contributions to medical psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 9–42). Oxford: Permagon Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar Ley P. (1979). Memory for medical information.British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology,, 18, 245–255. CrossrefGoogle Scholar Ley P. (1988). Communicating with patients. New York: Croom Helm. Google Scholar Ley P., Bradshaw P. W., Eaves D., & Walker C. M. (1973). A method for increasing patients’ recall of information presented by doctors.Psychological Medicine,, 3, 217–220. CrossrefGoogle Scholar Martin F. N., Krueger J. S., & Bernstein M. (1990). Diagnostic information transfer to hearing-impaired adults.Texas Journal of Audiology and Speech Pathology,, 16, 29–32. Google Scholar Reynolds P. M., Sanson-Fisher R. W., Poole A. D., Harker J., & Byrne M. J. (1981). Cancer and communication: Information-giving in an oncology clinic.British Medical Journal,, 282, 1449–1451. CrossrefGoogle Scholar Scheitel S. M., Boland B. J., Wollan P. C., & Silverstein M. D. (1996). Patient-physician agreement about medical diagnoses and cardiovascular risk factors in the ambulatory general medical examination.Mayo Clinic Proceedings,, 71, 1131–1137. CrossrefGoogle Scholar Shapiro D. E., Boggs S. R., Melamed B. G., & Graham-Pole J. (1992). The effect of varied physician affect on recall, anxiety, and perceptions in women at risk for breast cancer: An analogue study.Health Psychology,, 11, 61–66. CrossrefGoogle Scholar Starfield B., Steinwachs D., Morris I., Bause G., Siebert S., & Westin C. (1979). Patient-doctor agreement about problems needing follow-up visit.JAMA,, 242, 344–346. CrossrefGoogle Scholar Starfield B., Wrau C., Hess K., Gross R., Birk P. S., & D’Lugoff B. C. (1981). The influence of patient-practitioner agreement on outcome of care.American Journal of Public Health,, 71, 127–131. CrossrefGoogle Scholar Thomson A. M., Cunningham S. J., & Hunt N. P. (2001). A comparison of information retention at an initial orthodontic consultation.European Journal of Orthodontics,, 23, 169–178. CrossrefGoogle Scholar Tuckett D., Boulton M., Olson C., & Williams A. (1985). Meetings between experts: An approach to sharing ideas in medical consultations. London: Tavistock Publications. Google Scholar Audiology Counseling Literature Clark J. G., & Martin F. N. (1994). Effective counseling in audiology: Perspectives and practice.: Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Google Scholar Crowe T. A. (Ed.). (1997). Applications of counseling in speech-language pathology and audiology. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins. Google Scholar Erdman S. A. (2000). Counseling adults with hearing impairment.In Alpiner J. G. and McCarthy P. A. (Eds.), Rehabilitative audiology: Children and adults (pp. 435–470). Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Williams. Google Scholar Flasher L. V., & Fogle P. T. (2004). Counseling skills for speech-language pathologists and audiologists. Clifton Park, NY: Delmar Learning. Google Scholar Kricos P. B. (2000). Family counseling for children with hearing loss.In Alpiner J. G. and McCarthy P. A. (Eds.), Rehabilitative audiology: Children and adults (pp. 275–305). Philadelphia: Lippincott, Williams & Williams. Google Scholar Hodgson W. R. (1994). Audiologic counseling.In Katz J. (Ed.), Handbook of clinical audiology (pp. 616–623). Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins. Google Scholar Luterman D. M. (1987). Counseling parents of hearing-impaired children.In Martin F. N. (Ed.), Hearing disorders in children (pp. 309–319). Austin: Pro-Ed. Google Scholar Luterman D. M. (2001). Counseling persons with communication disorders and their families. (4th ed.). Austin: Pro-Ed. Google Scholar Madell J. R. (1998). Behavioral evaluation of hearing in infants & young children. New York: Thieme Medical Publishers. Google Scholar Madell J. R. (2000). Counseling for diagnosis and management of auditory disorders in infants, children, and adults.In Valente M., Hosford-Dunn H., & Roeser R. J. (Eds.), Audiology: Treatment (pp. 291–305). New York: Thieme Medical Publishers. Google Scholar Mencher G. T. (1996). Counseling families of hearing-impaired children: suggestions for the audiologist.In Gerber S. E. (Ed.), The handbook of pediatric audiology (pp. 343–351). Washington DC: Gallaudet University Press. Google Scholar Sanders D. E. (1975). Hearing aid orientation and counseling.In Pollack M. C. (Ed.), Amplification for the hearing impaired (pp. 323–372). New York: Grune and Stratton. Google Scholar Author Notes Robert H. Margolis, is professor of audiology at the University of Minnesota School of Medicine. In 2000 he started Audiology Incorporated to develop and market education materials for hearing-impaired patients and their families and to obtain federal funding for the development of automated hearing tests. The “Understanding …” series of patient education materials can be viewed at the Audiology Incorporated Web site. Advertising Disclaimer | Advertise With Us Advertising Disclaimer | Advertise With Us Additional Resources FiguresSourcesRelatedDetailsCited byAmerican Journal of Audiology30:3 (590-601)10 Sep 2021Targeted Re-Instruction for Hearing Aid Use and Care SkillsCarly C. M. Alicea and Karen A. DohertyThe ASHA Leader11:4 (8-33)1 Mar 2006The Counseling RelationshipDavid Luterman Volume 9Issue 14August 2004 Get Permissions Add to your Mendeley library History Published in print: Aug 1, 2004 Metrics Current downloads: 763 Topicsasha-topicsleader_do_tagasha-article-typesleader-topicsCopyright & Permissions© 2004 American Speech-Language-Hearing AssociationLoading ..." @default.
- W799073868 created "2016-06-24" @default.
- W799073868 creator A5040435232 @default.
- W799073868 date "2004-08-01" @default.
- W799073868 modified "2023-09-26" @default.
- W799073868 title "Boosting Memory With Informational Counseling" @default.
- W799073868 cites W1966202110 @default.
- W799073868 cites W1975079199 @default.
- W799073868 cites W1976118167 @default.
- W799073868 cites W2006038732 @default.
- W799073868 cites W2006991992 @default.
- W799073868 cites W2033906309 @default.
- W799073868 cites W2043630190 @default.
- W799073868 cites W2080849181 @default.
- W799073868 cites W2092730173 @default.
- W799073868 cites W2120233601 @default.
- W799073868 cites W2146928699 @default.
- W799073868 cites W2150754706 @default.
- W799073868 cites W2480515184 @default.
- W799073868 cites W4238333957 @default.
- W799073868 cites W4297679336 @default.
- W799073868 doi "https://doi.org/10.1044/leader.ftr5.09142004.10" @default.
- W799073868 hasPublicationYear "2004" @default.
- W799073868 type Work @default.
- W799073868 sameAs 799073868 @default.
- W799073868 citedByCount "13" @default.
- W799073868 countsByYear W7990738682012 @default.
- W799073868 countsByYear W7990738682014 @default.
- W799073868 countsByYear W7990738682015 @default.
- W799073868 countsByYear W7990738682018 @default.
- W799073868 countsByYear W7990738682021 @default.
- W799073868 countsByYear W7990738682022 @default.
- W799073868 crossrefType "journal-article" @default.
- W799073868 hasAuthorship W799073868A5040435232 @default.
- W799073868 hasConcept C138885662 @default.
- W799073868 hasConcept C154945302 @default.
- W799073868 hasConcept C15744967 @default.
- W799073868 hasConcept C180747234 @default.
- W799073868 hasConcept C2781344122 @default.
- W799073868 hasConcept C2911179318 @default.
- W799073868 hasConcept C41008148 @default.
- W799073868 hasConcept C41895202 @default.
- W799073868 hasConcept C46686674 @default.
- W799073868 hasConcept C70410870 @default.
- W799073868 hasConceptScore W799073868C138885662 @default.
- W799073868 hasConceptScore W799073868C154945302 @default.
- W799073868 hasConceptScore W799073868C15744967 @default.
- W799073868 hasConceptScore W799073868C180747234 @default.
- W799073868 hasConceptScore W799073868C2781344122 @default.
- W799073868 hasConceptScore W799073868C2911179318 @default.
- W799073868 hasConceptScore W799073868C41008148 @default.
- W799073868 hasConceptScore W799073868C41895202 @default.
- W799073868 hasConceptScore W799073868C46686674 @default.
- W799073868 hasConceptScore W799073868C70410870 @default.
- W799073868 hasIssue "14" @default.
- W799073868 hasLocation W7990738681 @default.
- W799073868 hasOpenAccess W799073868 @default.
- W799073868 hasPrimaryLocation W7990738681 @default.
- W799073868 hasRelatedWork W1985950550 @default.
- W799073868 hasRelatedWork W2042940951 @default.
- W799073868 hasRelatedWork W2050944115 @default.
- W799073868 hasRelatedWork W2065074698 @default.
- W799073868 hasRelatedWork W2079107071 @default.
- W799073868 hasRelatedWork W2085642841 @default.
- W799073868 hasRelatedWork W2093287941 @default.
- W799073868 hasRelatedWork W2118417921 @default.
- W799073868 hasRelatedWork W2128524530 @default.
- W799073868 hasRelatedWork W2513428240 @default.
- W799073868 hasVolume "9" @default.
- W799073868 isParatext "false" @default.
- W799073868 isRetracted "false" @default.
- W799073868 magId "799073868" @default.
- W799073868 workType "article" @default.